Placeholder Image

字幕表 動画を再生する

  • >>Male Presenter: Kyle Johnson is here to talk about "Inception and Philosophy." He's

  • the editor of the book, among--. He's also the frequent contributor to other volumes

  • of the Blackwell Philosophy and Popular Culture series, including Heroes in Philosophy. And

  • he's here to talk today about Inception and why--. What's the premise of the--?

  • >>Kyle Johnson: It should've won Best Picture.

  • >>Male Presenter: Why Inception should've won Best Picture. It's a very cogently argued

  • philosophical argument, which I think you will all enjoy. And without further ado, let's

  • welcome Kyle to Google.

  • [applause]

  • >>Kyle Johnson: Thank you very much, Tyler. So, just to make sure, who's seen Inception?

  • Good. All right. 'Cause if you haven't seen it, you almost can't spoil Inception because

  • it's unclear what's going on in Inception, right? So, there's no ending to spoil necessarily.

  • But what I do wanna argue today is that Inception should have won Best Picture. And I should

  • warn you that there's gonna be a lot of stuff flying at you here. The words on the slides

  • are really for my benefit. The pictures are for your benefit, so don't feel like you need

  • to read everything.

  • The Power Point is designed such that you can just go through the Power Point by yourself

  • and understand it. So, don't be overwhelmed by some text-heavy slides as it were. But

  • I'm going to argue that Inception should've won Best Picture. Now, I really don't actually

  • care that much about whether or not it won Best Picture.

  • Like, I wasn't crying the night the Oscars were on, whenever it didn't win. But the reason

  • I think it didn't win is because the Academy didn't understand it. I think it went right

  • over their heads.

  • [laughter]

  • And so what I'm really attempting to do here is I'm attempting to explain, by telling you

  • why it should've won, I'm going to explain the movie. I'm gonna help you understand the

  • movie about what it was about, about even what happened in it. Like, what actually is

  • going on in the plot.

  • And I'm gonna show you how philosophy can help you understand the movie. And I think

  • maybe you can even really truly understand the movie without it. And so, that's a lot

  • of what the book tries to do is help you use philosophy to understand Inception. And then,

  • once you understand it, we go off and we explore other philosophical issues that are raised

  • by the movie.

  • So, some things that they may have missed. One thing that the Academy probably missed

  • about Inception was that the movie itself is an analogy--it's an allegory--for movie-making.

  • That the dream team, each element of the dream team has an analogous element to those who

  • make a movie.

  • So, Cobb, who orchestrates everything, he's the director. Ariadne, who designs the dreams,

  • she's the screen-writer. Saito, who bankrolls the whole thing, who buys the whole airline

  • instead of just buying out first class, he's the production company. He's the bankroll.

  • Arthur, who organizes everything. He's the producer. Eames, who puts on characters--literally

  • portrays the sexy blonde--or Browning, the Godfather, he's the actor. Yusuf, who has

  • the technical savvy to chemically concoct the chemical they use to put themselves under

  • to make the whole thing possible, he's special effects. Fischer, the mark, he's the audience.

  • And we even see things like this, where we see Eames as Browning. You see Eames in the

  • mirror there. He's actually sitting at an old-time vanity mirror like an actor would.

  • And so, we have this direct analogy with movie-making itself where Inception is actually an analogy

  • for movie-making itself. Here's something else that they probably missed.

  • [music plays]

  • I believe it was Hans Zimmer who did the music for Inception, has admitted in interviews

  • that it's not just the intro. Every piece of music all the way throughout the film is

  • based on different parts of that Edith Piaf track, either sped up or slowed down to different

  • tempos.

  • And he just took those elements, took it, sped it up, slowed it down, and then composed

  • the music for the film based on that. That is cool. That is really cool and it's something

  • that most people missed about the film. And in fact, Inception itself is an inception.

  • You may think that Inception is impossible.

  • In fact, they even talk about that in the movie that it's impossible to get into someone's

  • mind an implant an idea in there and make them think that it was their own idea. But

  • that's just what movies do. That's all movies do is--. It's not all they do, but that's

  • one big thing that they do is they incept ideas into us.

  • Inception probably incepted into you the idea that reality may not be actually real, but

  • instead is a dream. Inception happens all the time. And that's what the whole point

  • of advertising is. Inception. And so, but these are not even--. This is just, this is

  • tawdry stuff.

  • This is just little tiny things that you may have missed. This is not even the big stuff.

  • Kinda cool, but not the big stuff. Here's the big stuff. Or at least, starting with

  • the big stuff. Just, just getting started. On the surface, the movie is a great action

  • film with some cool special effects and a clever cliffhanger.

  • At the end and he spins the top to see if he's really in reality and they fade to the--.

  • They go to the top. Is it gonna fall? And they cut out. You don't know. That's kinda

  • cool, right? Unraveling the movie would seem to simply require discovering the answer to

  • the question, "Did the top fall?"

  • And if you knew whether the top fell or not, then you'd know whether Cobb was home and

  • the movie can be nicely wrapped up. The first step to understanding Inception is realizing

  • that the answer to that question, "Did the top fall?" doesn't matter at all. Even if

  • we knew whether or not the top fell, we would still not understand the movie.

  • Even if the top falls, Cobb could still be dreaming. And in fact, I think he probably

  • is. Now, I'm gonna give you an argument for why. So first, we have to start out asking

  • ourselves how do totems work. 'Cause Cobb's top was not the only totem in the movie.

  • Arthur's got a totem. It's the die. Ariadne's got a totem. It's the bishop. There's some

  • others as well. You're never supposed to let anyone else see how your totem works. You

  • don't even want anyone else to touch your totem. Because if they do, they might feel

  • how it's weighted in the real world and then your totem will not be able to tell you whether

  • or not you're in one of their dreams.

  • So for example, Arthur's totem is the loaded die. If Ariadne touched his totem, she might

  • get inkling about how it's weighted in the real world. And she would know that whenever

  • he rolls it in the real world, it always comes up a five. So, he can't let her touch that

  • because if she touches that, then if he's in one of her dreams and he rolls his die

  • in her dream--.

  • Well, she knows it's supposed to come up a five and so she would dream it would come

  • up a five. So, you can't let anyone know how your totem behaves in the real world. If she

  • does touch it, then it will not be able to tell Arthur whether or not he is in her dream.

  • This is why he doesn't let her touch it. This is also why Ariadne refuses to let Cobb touch

  • her totem, the bishop. If he gets an inkling as to how it works and how it's weighted in

  • the real world and how it falls, then it won't be able to tell her whether or not he's in

  • one of his dreams.

  • So since, but here's the thing. Most importantly, what this means is that totems can only tell

  • you that you're not in someone else's dream. Arthur even specifically says that in the

  • film. It can only tell you if you're in someone else's dream. It can't tell you whether or

  • not you're in your own dream.

  • So, even if the top falls in the end, Cobb could still be dreaming because he could still

  • be in his own dream because he knows how his totem works. So even if it falls, he could

  • still be in his own dream. But it gets worse. Cobb reveals too much.

  • When Ariadne calls totems an "elegant solution for keeping track of reality,"--this is right

  • after he'd asked to see hers and she said, "No, you can't see it." And he says, "Good

  • job. You shouldn't let anyone know how your totem works." Right after that, she says,

  • "It's an elegant solution for keeping track of reality."

  • And asked if it was his idea. And he says, "No, it was Mal's actually. This one was hers.

  • She would spin it in the dream and it would never topple, just spin and spin." He just

  • did what he told her never to do--tell people how your totem works. He just told her how

  • it works.

  • So now, the totem is no good for telling him whether or not he's in one of her dreams because

  • now she knows how it works. And since she designed all the dreams of the inception,

  • it can't tell him whether or not he's out of the inception or not because tops would

  • fall.

  • She knows how it works in all the dreams in the inception. And worse yet, the top was

  • originally Mal's. That was her totem. It's not his. She knows how it works. So, it can't

  • tell him whether or not he's in her dream, either. So, even if the top falls at the end,

  • he could still be in his own dream.

  • He could still be in Ariadne's dream. He could still be in Mal's dream. Now, he thinks Mal

  • is dead of course, so he doesn't have to worry about that. But the problem, of course, is

  • she might have been right. And if she was, she's still alive. We'll talk more about that

  • in a little bit.

  • But it gets even worse. Those three people that it could be, that he's still dreaming

  • in, he's in their dream. But it gets worse. Think about how the other totems work. Arthur

  • only knows what number his die falls on in the real world. Only Ariadne knows how her

  • bishop is weighted in the real world.

  • There's another totem. Eames' totem, the poker chip. It's not exactly stated in the film,

  • but you can tell because he's always playing with it. That's his totem. And it's not quite

  • clear how it works, but if you think about it you can figure it out. There's one line

  • in the film where Cobb talks about the misspelling on his chip.

  • And, if you went to ComiCon this year, one of my contributors, Lance, showed me this

  • picture. This is from ComiCon this year. They had Eames' totem on display. And if you look,

  • it says "Mombasa District Casino, one hundred Shillings." It's a Mombasa Casino casino chip.

  • But it's misspelled. There's an extra "S" in Mombasa. And this is how his totem works.

  • If he looks at his poker chip and he sees that extra "S" he knows he's in the real world

  • 'cause he put it there. But if he looks at his poker chip and it's spelled correctly,

  • it doesn't have that extra "S."

  • Then he knows he's in someone else's dream. But with each one of these totems, notice

  • that their behavior in the real world is unique. It's loaded. It's weighted. It has an extra

  • "S." In the dream, it behaves ordinarily. Roll the die and it rolls random. But Cobb's

  • totem is backwards.

  • How does it behave in the real world? Like all tops behave in the real world. It falls

  • down. Its behavior in the dream is unique. All the other totems, how they behave in the

  • real world is unique, and in the dream is ordinary. His is ordinary in the real world,

  • unique in a dream.

  • It's backwards. And since not only do Mal and Cobb, obviously and Ariadne know that

  • his top would fall in the real world, we know his top would fall in the real world. That's

  • what tops do. Everybody knows that. If Cobb was in one of your dreams and he spun his

  • top, what would you dream that it would do?

  • Well, I'd dream that it would fall 'cause that's how I think they behave in the real

  • world. Cobb, even if the top falls at the end, he could still be in anyone's dream.

  • The top falling at the end tells us nothing. It is a red herring. It is there to distract

  • you to think you've got it figured out.

  • Oh, if I only knew if the top fell I'd have it all figured out. No, you wouldn't. [laughter]

  • And this is not a mistake. This is not an oversight. This is intentional. Cobb himself

  • is shown as an unreliable source of information in the film. "You notice how much time Cobb

  • spends doing the things he never says to do,” is actually a line from Arthur in the movie.

  • We see that. We actually see two versions of some of the events of the film that he

  • recounts. Like, whenever he is, when he and Mal are laying on the train track in Limbo,

  • the first time we see it, they're young. And then later, when we see it again, they're

  • old.

  • Well, which is it? Cobb tells us himself that he tries to alter his memories. What is he

  • saying? Is any of it accurate at all? Is that really how totems work? We don't know. That's

  • the beauty of it. That ending was much more clever than you thought. Much more clever

  • than you thought.

  • What's clever is the magic trick that no one pulls on us. No one has misdirected you, trying

  • to make you pay attention to the wrong thing--the top--to try to find out whether Cobb is still

  • dreaming. So, what you're doing at the end of the film is like, "Oh, I wonder if he's

  • still dreaming."

  • And so, you're looking down here at the top. Will it fall? Will it fall? Will it fall?

  • While you're looking, what's actually going--the clue--is up here on the upper-right. You need

  • to be watching and listening to those children whenever they first meet Cobb after he's back

  • home.

  • You need to be listening. That's where the clue is, but you didn't hear it 'cause you

  • were looking at this. Cobb has misdirected you. Now, to tell you. The children say something

  • here that's very illuminating. But to understand why it's illuminating, I need to give you

  • a little background.

  • And the background is this. We see in the movie that the subconscious works its way

  • through dreams. The most obvious example is the train in Limbo. The train from Limbo barreling

  • down in Yusuf's kidnap dream in the middle of that city street. The subconscious element,

  • part of Cobb's subconscious is working its way through a dream.

  • This is not the only example. Two really good examples. That random string of number that

  • Fischer arbitrarily gives as a combination to his father's safe. Right? They're in the

  • kidnap dream. He's like, "Now tell me the first five, the first six numbers that come

  • right to your head right now."

  • And he says, "Uhh, I don't know. Five, two, eight, four, nine, one." "You'll have to do

  • better than that." And they haul him off. Well, that five, two, eight, four, nine, one

  • starts showing up in the dream after that again and again and again. It's the combination

  • of both safes in Eames' snow fortress dream.

  • It's the fake telephone number that Eames gives as the sexy blonde and it's also in

  • Arthur's hotel dream. In addition, Mal and Cobb's anniversary suite number, where she

  • jumps from the window is three, five, oh, two. That number is also on the train that

  • barrels down through the middle of the street and the taxi they hail in that dream is two,

  • zero, five, three.

  • It's that same number backwards. So, there's a napkin. Five, two, eight, four, nine, one.

  • That's the phone number that the sexy blonde gives Fischer. There's the hotel room numbers

  • in Arthur's hotel room dream. Five, two, eight, and four, nine, one. And then if you were

  • to look at Fischer put in the combination for the safe, you see there five, two, eight,

  • four, nine, one for the safe that is in the snow fortress.

  • It's hard to see here 'cause the picture isn't very good, but you'd see three, five, oh,

  • two here on the door. Especially if you have it in Blu-Ray, you can see the three. five,

  • oh, two there. You can see the three, five, oh, two on the train there, as it barrels

  • down through the middle of the city street.

  • And then, the two, zero, five, three on the taxi that they hailed in that same dream.

  • Subconscious elements worked their way through. Well, another subconscious element has worked

  • its way through. Both at the beginning and ending of the film, we see that Saito dreams

  • of a mansion on an ocean--a house, as it is described in the script, "a house on a cliff."

  • When Cobb returns to his children at the end of the film and asks them what they have been

  • doing, they say, "They are building a house on a cliff." Turn the captions on and you'll

  • see it right there in black and white. It looks like a subconscious element of Saito--Saito's

  • subconscious--is working its way through into the dream that is at the end of the film.

  • It's working its way through. Peeking out that subconscious. Now, why think that Cobb

  • is in Saito's dream specifically? Not his own dream or someone else's dream? Well, here's

  • the thing. Think about where, if you exit Limbo. If you commit suicide in Limbo, where

  • do you go?

  • Well, we only actually have two examples in the film of where you go when you exit Limbo.

  • And that's Ariadne and Fischer. At the end of the film, they go down there to find Fischer.

  • She kicks him off the building. He falls and he wakes up. And a little bit later, she throws

  • herself off.

  • She falls. She wakes up. But where did they go? Back to the real world? No. They go one

  • layer up. They go to the snow fortress dream--Eames' snow fortress dream. They go one layer up

  • and then Fischer finishes the inception there. And when Ariadne gets back, they ride the

  • kicks back up the layers.

  • But you only go one layer up when you exit Limbo, not back to the real world--not all

  • the way back to the front. One layer up. That's it. So, at the end of the film--you can barely

  • see the picture there--but at the end of the film, whenever Saito, when Cobb finds Saito

  • in Limbo and he's got his gun, if he were to shoot himself in the head, where would

  • he go?

  • Or wouldn't he go where everyone else goes when they exit Limbo, one layer up. And that

  • would be to Eames' snow fortress dream. But everyone's already left that dream layer.

  • So, he would find it empty, ready for the taking. He would fill it with his own expectations,

  • his own assumptions, his own subconscious, and that would be to find himself on the plane

  • after the inception was complete.

  • Once Cobb shoots himself after that, he would pop up to that same level, find Saito's airplane

  • dream and would go back to his kids in that airplane dream. And notice that they could

  • be there for ten years--the way dreams work--before they finally realize that they were still

  • dreaming.

  • So it's entirely possible, based on a consistent interpretation of the film, that Cobb was

  • still dreaming. Even if the top fell he would still be dreaming. He'd be in Saito's dream

  • as a dream that he created once he got Eames' snow fortress dream layer that was empty once

  • he woke up from Limbo only going one layer up.

  • So, Inception is more complicated than you think. What's clever about the ending is not

  • the fact that it's a clever cliffhanger. It's clever because it tricked you into thinking

  • it was a clever cliffhanger when it wasn't a cliffhanger at all. You should've already

  • suspected that he was still dreaming and realize that the top was a red herring.

  • That's what's clever about it. Nolan misdirected it. At first, you were confused. Then, you

  • thought you had it figured out. But then, you start to think about it and everything

  • you thought you figured out, you're not confused about, you actually misunderstood. That's

  • what's beautiful about it.

  • It lends itself to multiple interpretations. Are we convinced that it was better than The

  • King's Speech yet?

  • [laughter]

  • Come on. But we've only scratched the surface. If we think about Saito and where you go when

  • you exit Limbo, if when you exit Limbo you just go one layer up, like Ariadne and Fischer

  • did.

  • Then where did Mal and Cobb go when they exited Limbo, when they put their head on the train

  • tracks and they exited Limbo? Would they have done what everybody else does? Go one layer

  • up? Well, what would be on that layer? Would that be the real world?

  • Well, Cobb actually tells Ariadne, whenever he's recounting the events that preceded their

  • meeting, that they entered Limbo after experimenting with--. Let me get the quote exactly right

  • here. "After exploring the concept of a dream within a dream." They were doing multi-level

  • dreaming and he pushed them too far.

  • He went too deep and they landed in Limbo. But that means that they entered Limbo after

  • going through a multi-level dream. So, when they woke up from Limbo, where would they

  • have gone? Would they have just gone one layer up of that multi-level dream? We see them

  • awake on this apartment floor hooked up to a passive device, but is that the real world

  • or wouldn't that just be the lowest level of the multi-level dream they used to get

  • into Limbo in the first place?

  • The real world in which the whole plot of the movie takes place could actually be a

  • dream. Maybe the whole movie is a dream from beginning to end. Forget about the end. The

  • whole movie looks like it may be a dream. And in fact, Nolan leaves us many clues that

  • suggest exactly this.

  • So, if you look at the Mombasa chase scene that's supposed to happen in the real world,

  • it has very many dream-like elements. The overhead shots establish that Mombasa is like

  • a maze. The agents that are after him literally pop in and out of nowhere inexplicably.

  • And the walls of buildings literally close in around him just like they do in dreams.

  • So, we see that it Mombasa is like a maze. We see Cobol agents that come out of nowhere.

  • It's a blurry pic, but if you look at when he's in the cafe and he gets called out and

  • he starts to run, literally from out of nowhere, there's an agent that comes and tackles him

  • from the right.

  • There's no way. He was just there. Literally appearing out of nowhere [whispering] just

  • like they do in dreams. And the walls close in around him. They look like they're fine

  • here and as I try to go through it squeezes and squeezes and the walls literally are closing

  • in around me. Just like they do in dreams.

  • Eames--clue number two--Eames is a dream forger, but looks like he forges in reality. Eames

  • as a dream forger, appearing as others in dreams and magically lifting Fischer's wallet

  • in Arthur's hotel room dream as the sexy blonde. If you look closely, whenever he's lifting

  • his wallet in the hotel room dream, he doesn't actually touch him.

  • He doesn't actually get anywhere close to him. He just has the wallet. It just appears.

  • Which is fine. He's dreaming. He can do that. He's a dream forger. He can just forge the

  • wallet. Yet in the real world, Eames forges casino chips and he lifts Fischer's passport

  • in the airplane in exactly the same way.

  • So, Eames picks pockets in the real world just like he does in a dream without even

  • touching him. Watch the scene and you'll see he can't even come close. It's like, Eames

  • is there. They're sort of close. And the poof, he has the passport inexplicably. Eames bets

  • his last two chips in the real world and the script calls them his last two chips.

  • And he's broke. He even says, "You've gotta buy if we're gonna bring over beer. You gotta

  • buy." And then he goes to the cashier and just magically poof, here's chips. Cashes

  • them in. He's literally dream-forging right there in the real world. The script even describes

  • it.

  • He mysteriously produces two stacks of chips that he then cashes in. Clue number three.

  • Mal's suicide. You can't see it here. Consider where Mal sits during her suicide attempt.

  • What supposedly happened was she trashed their hotel suite and then climbed out on the ledge.

  • But if she did that, she would be on the same side of the building as their room. He would

  • be able to look out the window and look and she would be out there on their side of the

  • building. That's not where she's at. She's in the window of another hotel room across

  • the way.

  • And it is another hotel room. If you look behind her, you'll see the same things that

  • are behind Cobb. It's another hotel. I mean, the window of another hotel room. That doesn't

  • make any sense. How did she get over there? And in fact, Cobb doesn't even realize it

  • doesn't make sense.

  • He's asking her, "Please come back in. Come back in." As she can just walk across that

  • gap? It doesn't make any sense. That's exactly the kind of thing that you watch and you don't

  • really think about it, but then you think about it a little bit later and that doesn't

  • make much sense.

  • Just like in a dream. Weird things happen in dreams and they seem perfectly normal,

  • but then when you wake up you go, "Yeah, that didn't make much sense. How did I not know

  • that I wasn't dreaming? How did I not know that I was dreaming?" How do you not know

  • that he wasn't dreaming right there?

  • He's gotta be. His father-in-law, Miles, even tells him to "come back to reality," at one

  • point. And this is my favorite clue. The song the dreamers use to herald the end of a movie

  • is that Edith Piaf song that we listened to before. It means "No, I regret nothing" in

  • English.

  • When the song is done, the dream is over. That's what heralds the end of the dreams.

  • The song is done, dream is over. The running time of the original recording of that song

  • that they use in the film is two minutes and 28 seconds. Inception is, to the second, two

  • hours and 28 minutes long.

  • Exactly. Watch your Blu-Ray player. Watch it click down. Exactly two hours and 28 minutes

  • long. Could it be, just like with shared dreaming, when the movie is done and the song is done,

  • the dream is over? The entire movie, I think, is a dream. Now, the thing is, you can't--.

  • There's always two sides to every coin. No clue is gonna settle this one way or the other.

  • And there are clues that suggest that the real world is indeed real. This is a good

  • Nolan--. Anybody know where this is from? It's a Batman reference 'cause no one does

  • Batman

  • Two. But there's two sides to every coin. Let's look at a couple of clues that the real

  • world may actually be real. So for example, if you look at Cobb's kids, whenever he's

  • flashing back they're younger. And at the end, they're actually wearing slightly different

  • clothes and they're older.

  • And they're actually played by different actors and actresses. Two different actors there.

  • So, some may suggest that they really did age and he's back in the real world. Others

  • have suggested that Cobb's totem is not really the top. It's his wedding ring and that whenever

  • he's in the real world he doesn't wear the wedding ring, except the flashbacks.

  • And then when he's dreaming, he's still wearing the wedding ring. And this includes the end

  • of the movie. If you look at the end of the movie, whenever he's checking in with the

  • ISA agent, he's not wearing his wedding ring. That's gonna indicate that the end of the

  • movie is also real.

  • But the truth is, pointing to the movie and clues in the movie is never going to settle

  • anything. The movie is ambiguous and Nolan, himself, has admitted that he intentionally

  • made it ambiguous. It's supposed to be open to interpretation. Nothing will definitively

  • prove anything one way or the other.

  • The dream clues could merely indicate that Cobb is losing his grip on reality. But now

  • the dream clues could merely reflect Cobb's assumption that he's not dreaming when he

  • really is. The answer to the question of whether or not the entire movie is a dream is what

  • philosophers would call "under-determined."

  • There's not enough evidence there to settle the issue. But this is where philosophy can

  • come to the rescue. Philosophers and scientists know how to deal with under-determination.

  • For example, any scientific data can be accounted for by many possible hypotheses.

  • But we're not just stuck. We have ways of delineating and deciding which ones we should

  • prefer. Scientists prefer the most adequate hypothesis, the one that's most fruitful and

  • simple and wide-scoping and conservative.

  • [Kyle Johnson coughs]

  • Excuse me. This is what we did whenever we were debating about the heliocentric versus

  • the geocentric view of the universe, or the solar system at least. Is the sun the center

  • or the Earth the center? Well, the Earth being the center required all these weird retrogrades

  • and planets were revolving around points and blah, blah, blah.

  • And it's really complicated. Or this was simple. They all go around the sun. Very simple. And

  • so, we ended up selecting that, after we killed a few people. Apart from that--

  • [laughter]

  • we ended up selecting this even before we could experimentally identify that it was

  • definitely the right one as opposed to this because it was simpler, because it was more

  • adequate. And so, philosophers--. We really can't do that with an interpretation of Inception.

  • And philosophers have more guns in our arsenal. Philosophers, when presented with ambiguity,

  • like ambiguous statements--that kind of stuff--we employ the principle of charity. When it's

  • unclear what someone means, you choose the most charitable interpretation--the one that

  • entails the speaker is not an idiot, or not misinformed.

  • So, which interpretation of Inception is more charitable? Which one makes it a better movie?

  • I think it's the all-dream interpretation that makes it better. And the reason why is

  • because if it's not all a dream, there's some significant criticisms that can be leveled

  • against the movie.

  • For one, all of the characters, except for Cobb, are completely one-dimensional. Arthur,

  • Ariadne, Fischer, Saito. They don't even have last names, much less a past. They're all

  • just there for Cobb. They just do what Cobb wants them to do. Even Ariadne, who shows

  • just a little sliver of free will when she initially rejects the idea of being an architect--"I'm

  • outta here. I can't share my subconscious with someone like you."--and she walks out.

  • Cobb just says, "Oh, she'll be back." And then what does she do? The next thing? She

  • comes back. They're completely one-dimensional. They're only there for Cobb. That's not good

  • writing and Nolan doesn't usually do one-dimensional characters. Even, somebody gives me Batman's,

  • his butler.

  • >>MALE #1: Alfred.

  • >>Kyle Johnson: Alfred. Right. Even Alfred's got a past and he's a complicated character

  • in Batman. That's not Nolan's style. The editing in the real world is sloppy. There's quick

  • jumps from here to there and you're not quite sure how you got from here to there and why

  • are they doing this now?

  • There's all these really weird jumps in the real world in regards to just mere editing.

  • And that chase scene in Mombasa and he's got all the agents on him and then Saito shows

  • up out of nowhere. What are you doing in Mombasa? "I had to protect my investment." Really?

  • That's a little cheesy. That's not exactly the best way out of that situation. But if

  • it's all a dream, the characters are one-dimensional because they're just projections of Cobb's

  • subconscious. They each represent a different aspect of Cobb. And if you watch the movie

  • with that in mind, you'll see that each one of them plays a different role in his subconscious.

  • One's the planner. One's more daring. One's the moral conscious. You can even divide it

  • in id, ego, and superego. You see all of these elements. The sloppy editing? Well, we see

  • that same sloppy editing when we know that Cobb is dreaming. You jump from place to place

  • to place in a dream not realizing how you got there because that's what you do in dreams.

  • He's doing the same thing in the real world, jumping from place to place to place. And

  • yeah, that Saito line is a little bit cheesy--I have to protect my investment. But as a subtle

  • clue that Cobb is actually dreaming, that is brilliant. A much more charitable interpretation.

  • Now, you might think that it's not too charitable 'cause if the whole movie is a dream, well

  • then why would you care? Why would I want to watch a movie about a dream? Nothing's

  • really happing so I don't really care. Well, that's the thing. It's a movie. It's fiction.

  • Yeah, it doesn't really happen 'cause it's a dream. It doesn't happen anyway. It's a

  • movie.

  • [laughter]

  • Why would you care more about a movie about a dream than about a fictional movie about

  • events that didn't happen anyway? They all didn't happen. This is the paradox of fiction

  • that Tyler was talking about a little while ago.

  • Why do we care about events that we don't know are happening? Well, I'm not exactly

  • sure how to solve that paradox, but I know that the paradox arises whether or not the

  • movie is about a dream or not. And so, it doesn't make it a worse movie.

  • In fact, it makes it pretty cool 'cause it could be a metaphorical story about how a

  • disturbed mind handles its own dementia. I mean, there's all kinds of cool interpretations

  • that can go with it I think that are really, really interesting. Now, you might wonder

  • if we could solve all this if we just asked Nolan himself.

  • [laughter]

  • Is the whole movie a dream or not? And Nolan's even said that he does have a view. Like,

  • I approached him with a certain interpretation in mind and I know what I think is real and

  • what's not.

  • But does that matter? Does Nolan get to dictate how his film must be interpreted? Or, if he

  • makes it ambiguous, is it open to us? If he wanted it to be interpreted a certain way,

  • he had to put in something there to make it be interpreted that way. And if he intentionally

  • makes it ambiguous, then my interpretation is just as valid as his.

  • Is that the way art works? Or does authorial intention matter? That's what the first chapter

  • of my book is about. It raises this issue about whether or not the entire movie is a

  • dream and then talks about whether or not the authorial intention view is correct. Inception

  • should have won Best Picture.

  • Either the Academy didn't understand it or they didn't interpret it charitably. If they

  • had done either, they would have realized that it was much better than a film about

  • a stuttering English monarch. Clearly a better film. But even though it didn't win Best Picture,

  • Inception still wins Plato's Academy Award--it looks like Rodin's Thinker--because of its

  • philosophical depth, because of the plethora of philosophical questions that it raises

  • and, of course, I tackle in my book, "Inception and Philosophy: Because It's Never Just a

  • Dream,” published by Wiley Blackwell.

  • So, some of the other--not all of them--but some of the other topics that we cover in

  • the book. If we can't tell whether or not Cobb is dreaming, can we tell whether we're

  • dreaming... right now? Could this be a dream? Can you be certain? The answer is no, you

  • can't.

  • This is a classic, philosophical, skeptical problem. And once we realize we can't tell

  • for sure whether the real world is real, how do we deal with that angst? How do we deal

  • with the kind of tension, the kind of mental anguish that causes us? Coleman's got a chapter

  • about that.

  • Perhaps we should just have faith that the world is real. Maybe that's a way out of it.

  • But when is faith rational? Is faith ever rational? Faith is belief without evidence.

  • A lot of times, that's not rational. Like, I could believe without evidence that there's

  • an elephant behind me, but that's not rational.

  • When is faith, if ever, rational? Cobb doesn't think it's always rational. Mal asked him

  • to take a leap of faith right out that window and he refused. So, when is it rational to

  • take a leap of faith, if ever? That's what my chapter is about. Can you be held morally

  • responsible for what you do in your dreams?

  • You might think they don't have real-world impact, but what if you thought it was real?

  • Don't sometimes your intentions matter if you thought it was real and you had that chance

  • to cheat on your significant other in your dream and you did it? Aren't you a bit morally

  • culpable?

  • Wouldn't they be upset if they found out that's what you did in your dream? That's another

  • good chapter. Are real paradoxes, like the pin rose steps, possible? That's Tyler's chapter.

  • Is Inception really possible? Isn't that, for example, what advertisements do?

  • And if it is possible, what are the kind of ethics that go along with that? And does that

  • threaten free will? We don't think that Fischer gets moral responsibility. He doesn't freely

  • choose to break up his father's company. But if inception happens all the time in the real

  • world, are we morally responsible for what we do?

  • Do I really freely choose to eat that McDonald's hamburger when I'm bombarded with advertisements

  • all the time that make me want a McDonald's hamburger? What is time? What exactly is time

  • and can it really slow down in a dream, or speed up in a dream?

  • Would you really want to live in Limbo, a utopia, a perfect world? Or would you eventually

  • get bored with that? Are utopias even possible? Those are all issues that I talk about--that

  • I and my authors, of course--, my contributors talk about in the book. So, that's my presentation

  • and I thank you so much.

  • I'm ready for questions. I'd love to hear what you think.

  • [applause]

  • >>FEMALE #1: I'm just curious. You mentioned Freudian psychology or it sounded more Freudian,

  • but I have a writing teacher who really got enthralled with it. He actually has a really

  • good blog.

  • His name is Scott Myers and he was talking about it from a Jungian perspective with the

  • dream interpretation and it seems to lend itself a lot to that. I'm just wondering if

  • you get into that in your book at all.

  • >>Kyle Johnson: No. I'm sorry. I don't. I'm not familiar with it at all. I wish I would've

  • known about it so I could, but I don't.

  • >>FEMALE #1: Well, you might, since you're so into this, you might look into some Jungian

  • stuff. I can give you a list of books or whatever.

  • >>Kyle Johnson: Great.

  • >>FEMALE #1: Yeah, cool. OK.

  • >>Kyle Johnson: Cool. Thanks.

  • >>FEMALE #2: That's OK. I don't need a mic.

  • >>Male Presenter: You do for the recording.

  • >>FEMALE #2: OK. I was wondering if you had a link to the Power Point presentation at

  • all.

  • >>Kyle Johnson: I'm sure we can do that. I could--. I'll tell you what. When I get home,

  • I will post it on my website, so just google David Kyle Johnson. My webpage for King's

  • College will pop up and you can download it there. Cliff, can we make it available somehow

  • through--?

  • >>Cliff: Yeah.

  • >>FEMALE #2: OK. Awesome.

  • >>Kyle Johnson: Yeah, I'm hoping that I almost bombarded you a bit. And really, almost everything

  • I covered in there is in here. So.

  • >>FEMALE #3: So, how many times did you have to watch the movie to get all this?

  • >>Kyle Johnson: Yeah. That's a good question. So, quite a few times. I also took to just

  • watching specific parts as I was editing the book and seeing what I needed to see. A number

  • of other things were also, like--. Here's I think a really fun, cool part of the book.

  • If you add up all the times that all my contributors and I watched the movie, it's gotta be hundreds

  • and hundreds and hundreds of times. And one of the ways I took advantage of that was I

  • created a Google Doc--Google Docs are so cool--and I gave a link to every one of my contributors.

  • And I started a little appendix of cool things that you might have missed about the film.

  • And I let all my contributors just dump stuff on there. And after they were done, I went

  • through and edited.

  • And so, the end of the book is an appendix that is the result of like, hundreds and hundreds

  • and hundreds of watchings of the movie that have all these cool, little things that you

  • may have missed upon the first watching of the film. So, off the top of my head, a cool

  • one is the fact that Dom--one second--Dom, Robert, Eames, Arthur, Mal, Saito, put them

  • in the right order, they spell dreams.

  • Those kind of cool things. The stuff about Inception being analogy. There's all kinds

  • of stuff. We even have a catalog of exactly what those two kids were wearing before and

  • after, at the end of the movie and see exactly how different their outfits were. It's very,

  • very detailed. That's in the appendix. Go ahead.

  • >>MALE #2: So, about the kids. You didn't mention this specifically, but I suspect you

  • probably did turn it up that you never see the kid's faces until the end.

  • >>Kyle Johnson: Right. Until the end.

  • >>MALE #2: So, I don't know what the significance of that might be.

  • >>Kyle Johnson: Yeah. I mean, that could be a clue that he's really awake. It could be

  • just a clue that he thinks he's really gotten back home. And so, he can finally see his

  • kid's faces at the end. I mean, it could be a clue either way.

  • >>MALE #2: Cool. So, the other deeper question that I had is I read that clue about the wedding

  • ring months ago. I don't know if you read "The Last Psychiatrist."

  • >>Kyle Johnson: I don't think so.

  • >>MALE #2: It's a great blog. It's worth checking out. But he did a thing in there where he

  • mentioned it. And so, I was talking this over with a bunch of friends and one of them had

  • to watch the movie again. And his claim, the way he interpreted that, was that Cobb actually

  • destroyed his totem in the real world.

  • Because he knew it so well, he didn't need to actually have it present. He only knew

  • that it behaved the way he didn't expect it in the dream, then he was in someone else's

  • dream. And there's no reason to actually have it. And the whole thing about having Mal's

  • totem was just in remembrance of her and wasn't actually his totem.

  • >>Kyle Johnson: Interesting. So, he doesn't have a totem at all. So, his wedding ring's

  • not even his totem, or his wedding ring is his totem? He destroys it in the real world.

  • >>MALE #2: His wedding ring was--. He doesn't need it because he knows how it behaves and

  • he doesn't want anyone else to discover it, is one way of interpreting that.

  • >>Kyle Johnson: Right.

  • >>MALE #2: And I don't know if that's actually--. I don't know if that affects anything said

  • actually.

  • >>Kyle Johnson: I mean, it still doesn’t cause--. It still doesn't solve the problem

  • because he could still be in his own dream.

  • >>MALE #2: Yeah. But it's an interesting thing though about that.

  • >>Kyle Johnson: Yeah. Absolutely.

  • >>MALE #3: Did you circle back with Christopher Nolan to validate any of your observations?

  • >>Kyle Johnson: No, I haven't. I wish I could. I'd love to sit down and talk with Christopher

  • Nolan. I wasn't able to do that. We did scope through his interviews and that kind of stuff

  • to see what he said.

  • So like, one thing I know then--but this raises the issue of whether or not Arthur can determine

  • the meaning of his movie or not--one thing I know is, one other kind of dream clue is

  • the fact that the company that's after Cobb is Cobol. C-O-B-O-L. Cobol, Cobb, Cobol.

  • He's after himself? That really looks--. And Nolan himself said, "Yeah, that's just a coincidence.

  • We had to change the name of that company multiple times for legal reasons." It looks

  • like that's just a coincidence, not really a clue. Right? But again, maybe it could still

  • serve as a clue if the author doesn't get to determine the absolute meaning of his film.

  • But I wish--. If you've got some contacts, let me know and I'd love to sit down and talk

  • with him.

  • [laughter]

  • >> Male Presenter: Last question.

  • >> MALE #4: So, do you think the fact that Mal's and Edith Piaf was both played by Marion

  • Cotillard was also a clue?

  • >> Kyle Johnson: I don't think it was a clue. No one actually talked about that as well.

  • And he basically said something like, I don't read too much in this. It's kind of a cool

  • thing. But don't read too much into it. It's not--. But that is--. The actress who plays

  • Mal also plays Edith Piaf in the movie about her life. It's the same actress. She's pretty

  • cool.

  • >> Male Presenter: Very cool. We'll have some time afterwards for more specific Inception

  • and Dragon Tattoo questions. Thank you very much for speaking at Google.

  • >> Kyle Johnson: Thanks.

  • [Applause]

>>Male Presenter: Kyle Johnson is here to talk about "Inception and Philosophy." He's

字幕と単語

ワンタップで英和辞典検索 単語をクリックすると、意味が表示されます

A2 初級

Authors@Google: Kyle Johnson 'Inception and Philosophy' !

  • 89 3
    fisher に公開 2013 年 03 月 24 日
動画の中の単語