Placeholder Image

字幕表 動画を再生する

  • It’s 1961. Youre wandering around Stanford University, looking for a sandwich or something

  • when you happen to walk by a particular room in a particular lab, and see something a little

  • unnerving. Namely, you find a woman punching an inflatable clown named Bobo in the neck.

  • Over and over in its neck. This was the lab of legendary psychologist Albert Bandura,

  • and in 1961 he was studying one of the most important phenomena in psychology.

  • See, while the woman was throttling that big inflatable clown, there was a child watching

  • her. And after about ten minutes of observing this clown-beating display, the kid was taken

  • to a room full of fun toys, which were soon taken away, and then the frustrated kid was

  • left alone with Bobo, and Bandura watched what happened. And yeah, what happened was

  • kind of scary. Kids who watched the woman beating the clown were much more likely to

  • mimic her aggression -- kicking, punching, throwing, even attempting to maul Bobo with

  • a hammer. But other children who saw an adult playing nicely with the doll, or just ignoring

  • it, didn’t respond the same way in their frustration.

  • Bandura’s results may seem predictable now, but in those days, they challenged the dominant

  • behaviorist views that we talked about last week -- the views that learning is solely

  • about conditioning and association, rewards and punishments. Bandura’s research focused

  • on how learning can occur through observing and imitating someone else’s behavior. And

  • if that seems obvious to you, you have Bandura to thank for that. His research hastened the

  • evolution of 20th century experimental psychology from pure behaviorism into what we now know

  • as social-cognitive learning. While it was closely related to behaviorism, the social-cognitive

  • models added profoundly new dimensions to what Skinner and Watson and Pavlov had observed

  • in our feathered and furry friends. In other words, it showed us that -- just as there’s

  • more than one way to beat up a clown -- there’s way more than one way to learn.

  • [INTRO]

  • Last week we talked about the differences between classical and operant conditioning

  • in associative learning -- the kind of learning that comes from connecting different events

  • and stimuli. In classical conditioning, this means associating a stimulus with some kind

  • of involuntary response -- the whole dog slobbering at the sound of a bell phenomenon -- whereas

  • operant conditioning makes associations between stimulus and a voluntary behavior -- like

  • the rat pressing a lever to get delicious snacks, or jumping out of a cage to escape

  • an electrical shock.

  • And that’s all well and good, but if learning is the process of acquiring and retaining

  • new behavior and information, then Bandura’s experiments showed us that conditioning with

  • external rewards, punishments, or other stimuli isn’t the only way to do it. It’s hard

  • to deny that pretty much all animals are capable of learning certain things by association,

  • but critics of behaviorists like Pavlov, Watson, and Skinner had a problem with their assertions

  • that, when it came to learning, it didn’t matter much whether youre training rats,

  • pigeons, or people--it’s all the same. Because, lots of research has demonstrated that an

  • animal’s capacity for conditioning is actually limited by its biology.

  • Consider this scenario: Say I get a raging case of food poisoning after eating my head-weight

  • in raw oysters with my friend Bernice. I’m probably not going to want to touch oysters

  • again for a long, long time, because I associate their smell and taste with the smell and taste

  • they made when I waswhen they were coming back out, is what I’m trying to say. But,

  • that doesn’t mean that the sight of Bernice, or the sound of the sea shanties they were

  • playing at the restaurant would make me barf, because humans are, by our very nature, more

  • taste averse than we are sight or sound averse. On the other hand, sight-oriented animals,

  • like birds, may be biologically predisposed to avoid tainted food by sight, since that’s

  • how they hunt and forage. And presumably they go to restaurants that play better music.

  • Anyway, all of this tells us that species can more easily learn associations that help

  • them thrive or survive, and that not all associations are learned equally. It’s a lot easier to

  • teach a pigeon to peck an X on the ground to obtain a food reward than it is get it

  • to flap its wings to get that same reward, because pecking is a natural foraging behavior

  • for a pigeon. In the same way, it would be much harder for the bird to learn to peck

  • that X to avoid a shock, rather than to flap its wings to avoid the shock, because flying

  • away from danger is what pigeons naturally do.

  • Learned associations are even more complicated in humans of course, because what we learn

  • doesn’t only influence our behavior, it also shapes our attitudes. Our cognition -- that

  • is, our thoughts, perspectives, and expectations -- is important for learning, as is our social

  • context, as Bandura figured out. So, Pavlov-style conditioning experiments that ignore those

  • social-cognitive elements can really run into trouble. For example, someone under treatment

  • for an alcohol addiction may be given booze laced with a nauseating drug. According to

  • the pure classical conditioning model, that person would then equate booze with feeling

  • nasty.

  • But the brain can override this association if it’s aware that it’s the added drug,

  • and not the alcohol, is the thing that’s causing the illness. Sometimes we can think

  • our way out of intended associations. And by the same token, a person’s social context

  • - like, their friends, family traditions, or life stressors - can reinforce something

  • like alcohol consumption more than the nauseating pill could ever punish it.

  • Plus, we also do a lot of latent learning, like without even knowing it. Have you ever

  • been walking around a new city, someone stops you to ask directions, and you surprise yourself

  • by actually being able to tell that tourist how to get to the park? That’s because were

  • constantly developing cognitive maps, or mental representations of our surroundings, without

  • explicitly telling ourselves to do it. Weve all seen the experiments with in mazes: Well,

  • those show us that even rats develop these cognitive maps, figuring out how to get around,

  • even if there’s no reward at the end. And days later, when they finally do get food

  • at the end of the maze, they quickly demonstrate all that earlier latent learning by scuttling

  • through the maze as fast, or faster, than rats that had been rewarded all along.

  • So, learning isn’t just about associating a response with a consequence. There’s thinking

  • happening, too. And this kind of thinking is also a big part of observational learning,

  • which is basically learning by watching other people, or being influenced by them in other

  • ways. Because, you don’t need direct experience to learn. You can just pick up stuff up through

  • modeling -- not like modeling on the catwalk, I just mean observing and imitating specific

  • behaviors.

  • Rats, crows, pigeons, primates, and other animals learn through imitation. Chimps learn

  • how to use sticks to fish ants out of a nest this way. One study found rhesus macaques

  • were usually slow to make up after a fight unless they grew up watching more forgiving

  • older macaques, in which case they tended to make up more quickly. Of course we humans

  • learn A LOT from modeling -- I mean, most of our popular culture is based on it: new

  • slang, skinny jeans, foodie trends, pixie cuts -- theyre all racing around the globe

  • through observation and imitation. So it makes a lot of sense that social observation shapes

  • behavior, especially in children.

  • Which brings us back to Bobo. Again, the fact that we learn by imitating, even when we don’t

  • mean to, seems pretty intuitive, but until Bandura’s famous experiment, it hadn’t

  • been studied in a scientific way. I mean, these kids started abusing Bobo not just with

  • little toddler punches, but with hostile language and even using things, like toy guns, that

  • they previously had no interest in -- and all because they saw aggressive modeling in

  • action. And since Bandura’s time, technology has allowed us to peer even deeper into this

  • dynamic.

  • Neuroimaging in humans, for instance, has shown that when an individual watches someone

  • else, especially someone whom they relate to, receive an award or score a goal or something,

  • their own brain’s reward systems light up vicariously. Italian researchers found this

  • out pretty much by accident in the early 1990s: They were studying signals from key regions

  • in a lab monkey’s brain that were associated with planning and doing. Their brain-monitoring

  • device buzzed softly when the monkey did something like pick up a piece of fruit and eat it.

  • But one hot day, a researcher came back from lunch licking an ice cream cone, and suddenly

  • heard the animal’s brain monitor buzz -- the monkey was watching him, and his brain worked

  • as if it was actually doing the licking.

  • Many scientists suspect that this is the work of a previously unknown type of brain cell

  • called mirror neurons, which fire when a subject both performs an action, and when they observe

  • someone else doing it. Mirror neuron research is still relatively new, and were still

  • figuring them out, but combined with Bandura’s earlier work, it’s revealing a strong connection

  • between observation, imitation, and learning. So the takeaway here is: Models are important!

  • And not just Gisele and Antonio Sabato Junior. You can, if you want, observe and imitate

  • them; I’m just saying that observational, social learning starts really early, and parental

  • figures are powerful role models.

  • Positive, supportive, and loving models usually prompt similar behavior in others, just as

  • negative, aggressive modeling can spark antisocial effects. And, as well talk about later,

  • what we see and feel and learn as children is not easily displaced when were adults.

  • Literary giant George Bernard Shaw wrote, “Imitation is not just the sincerest form

  • of flattery - it's the sincerest form of learning.” And British statesman Lord Chesterfield once

  • said, “We are, in truth, more than half what we are by imitation.” Even if these

  • ideas were only half-true, they’d still be a powerful lesson on who you choose to

  • spend your time with, and how you choose to act.

  • If you learned anything by watching me today, hopefully it involved the limitations of classical

  • and operant conditioning, the basics of cognitive, observational, and social learning, a look

  • at mirror neurons, and how to beat up a Bobo doll.

  • Thanks for watching, especially to all of our Subbable subscribers, who make this whole

  • channel possible. If you’d like to sponsor an episode of Crash Course, get a special

  • decal, or even be animated into an upcoming episode, just go to subbable.com. This episode

  • was written by Kathleen Yale, edited by Blake de Pastino, and our consultant is Dr. Ranjit

  • Bhagwat. Our director and editor is Nicholas Jenkins, and the script supervisor is Michael

  • Aranda, who is also our sound designer, and the graphics team is Thought Café.

It’s 1961. Youre wandering around Stanford University, looking for a sandwich or something

字幕と単語

ワンタップで英和辞典検索 単語をクリックすると、意味が表示されます

B1 中級

ボボ・ビートダウン - クラッシュコース心理学#12 (The Bobo Beatdown - Crash Course Psychology #12)

  • 92 17
    Julian Demann に公開 2021 年 01 月 14 日
動画の中の単語