Placeholder Image

字幕表 動画を再生する

  • What kind of day do you think this woman is having?

  • And what’s going on here?

  • And probably it isn’t hard to imagine how these two are feeling, right?

  • That’s because most humans are really good at silently communicating and interpreting

  • a whole range of emotions using only facial muscles.

  • Whether it’s voluntary or involuntary, a simple curled lip, raised eyebrow,

  • or crinkled nose says a lot.

  • In fact, many psychologists think that some of our basic facial expressions

  • like the ones that express anger, fear, happiness, surprise, sadness, and disgust are innate

  • not learned, and are universal across cultures.

  • That’s because these basic expressions probably started out as practical reactions to stimuli,

  • and eventually became associated with emotions.

  • In his appropriately-named book The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals, published in 1872,

  • Darwin helped popularize the idea of universality:

  • that were born to express emotions in a certain way.

  • He suggested that we humans inherited our emotional expressions from our ancestors,

  • and that these expressions helped increase survival by facilitating communication in

  • social groups.

  • He also proposed that our expressions adapted to environmental stimuli,

  • something that I will come back to in a minute.

  • Darwin’s book even came with olden-day photos of people demonstrating facial expressions,

  • like these pictures of people trying their best to show what grief looks like.

  • In the late 1960s, psychologist Paul Ekman began testing Darwin’s universality idea

  • by traveling around and conducting lots of independent, cross-cultural studies.

  • In New Guinea, for example, he encountered an isolated culture that had never seen outsiders before.

  • He told subjects brief emotional stories likeold friends are coming to visit,” or

  • you just stepped on a rotten dead pig,” and he showed them a set of three different photographs of facial expressions.

  • Ekman found that they usually picked the expression that he expected them to associate with the

  • emotion in the story, like a frown for sadness.

  • So even though this culture was very isolated from ours, the population used the same basic

  • facial expressions.

  • Other studies have found that infants, as well as people who were born blind, also use

  • the same facial expressions, without being taught which expressions go with which emotions.

  • By the 1970s, Ekman and other researchers had started working on a more objective way

  • to measure these universal facial expressions, by documenting muscle activity on the face itself.

  • They compiled what’s known as the Facial Action Coding System, or FACS.

  • The system looks at muscle engagement, noting the intensity, duration, and asymmetry of an expression.

  • By comparing the muscle movements on someone’s face with what an expression is supposed to look like,

  • psychologists can use the FACS to help distinguish emotions. Like, telling real smiles from fake ones,

  • to detect if someone is lying, or to identify certain emotions in people who can’t express themselves verbally.

  • So it seems like weve probably evolved with some basic facial expressions built-in.

  • And a 2013 study out of Cornell University looked at why.

  • The researchers had their subjects make neutral, scared, and disgusted expressions, and then

  • measured how much light made it to their retinas with each expression.

  • And it turns out that facial expressions may have first evolved to help us better react

  • to our environments by controlling our sensory input, like the amount of light we let into

  • our eyes.

  • Say youre walking through the jungle, for example, and suddenly you hear a loud crash.

  • It scares you, and as an expression of fear sweeps across your face, you widen your eyes.

  • This immediately expands your field of vision, letting in more light, and heightening your

  • visual sensitivity to help you locate any danger.

  • But if, say, you accidentally step on a mound of mystery poo in the park, youll do something very different.

  • As you recoil in disgust, your eyes narrow, letting less light in as you sharpen your

  • focus to examine your soiled foot.

  • That classic disgusted nose-wrinkling also helps decrease the size of your nasal cavity

  • and let less air flow through it, presumably so you don’t have to smell as much of whatever

  • is giving you the nasties.

  • This suggests that how you feel actually shapes your perception of reality, and

  • how much light hits the back of your eye.

  • Some social communication may have evolved from these reactions to outside stimuli, which

  • also supports Darwin’s idea that basic facial expressions are universal.

  • Though there is definitely a learned component to our facial expressions, too.

  • If youve ever nodded politely during a relative’s political rant, when really you

  • felt like screaming inside, you know that we learn to control our faces and mask emotions in certain social situations.

  • So, many facial expressions are probably innate. But managing them is a whole different story.

  • Thank you for watching this episode of SciShow, which was brought to you by our patrons on Patreon,

  • thank you to all of you, so much for helping support this show. If you want

  • to become one of those people, if you aren't already, you can go to patreon.com/scishow.

  • And don’t forget to go to youtube.com/scishow and subscribe!

What kind of day do you think this woman is having?

字幕と単語

ワンタップで英和辞典検索 単語をクリックすると、意味が表示されます

B2 中上級

私たちの表情はどこから来ているのでしょうか? (Where Do Our Facial Expressions Come From?)

  • 261 25
    Joseph Yap に公開 2021 年 01 月 14 日
動画の中の単語