字幕表 動画を再生する
Here’s a riddle, my Hobbitses: What’s something we all experience, all the time,
that we can’t really measure, and barely have words to define? You can’t hold it
in your hand, or take a bite out of it. It isn’t something you learn or practice; it
just IS.
Consciousness. Every science has certain concepts that are
so fundamental, yet abstract, that we have a hard time finding the appropriate words
to describe them. Ask a physicist and they’ll tell you energy and space defy simple definitions.
Biologists know if something is alive, but have a harder time explaining what life actually
is. Ask a psychologist what consciousness is, and you’ll get…you’ll get a slippery
answer. For the purposes of this conversation, we’re
going to actually loosely define consciousness as our awareness of ourselves and our environment.
It’s this awareness that allows us to take in and organize information from many sources
and senses, at once. American psychologist William James thought
of consciousness as a continuously moving, shifting, and unbroken stream, hence the term
“stream of consciousness.” Others think of it as the brain’s roving flashlight,
shining down an unbroken beam of light that highlights one thing, and then moves on to
the next. The point is, your conscious experience is
forever shifting--for example, right now hopefully you’re focused on the words coming out of
my mouth, but with a little shift - your mind might wander to how you really should shower
today, and your chair is uncomfortable, and you suddenly have to pee, and can you believe
what Bernice said?!
Do I smell pizza?
HEY! EYES HERE! WE’RE LEARNING! Beyond that moment-to-moment shifting, consciousness
allows us to contemplate life, think about infinity, and ride a unicycle across a tightrope
while juggling melons, at least in theory.
Our consciousness helps us plan our futures, consider consequences, and reflect on the
past. It is both the most familiar, and the most mysterious part of our lives.
It’s kind of like The Force -- but for the little universes inside our heads.
[INTRO] Throughout our daily lives we flit back and
forth between various states of consciousness, including waking, sleeping, and various altered
states. These can occur spontaneously, like dreaming, or be physiologically sparked, like
a drug-induced hallucination, or be triggered psychologically, through meditation or hypnosis,
for example.
We’re going to take the next three episodes to look closely at these different states
of consciousness, but let’s start with what it really means to be awake.
For centuries, scientists learned what they could about the brain solely through clinical
observation. And they learned a lot, for sure, but with today’s technology, we’re actually
able to see some of the structures and activity inside a living, working brain - its electrical,
metabolic, and magnetic signatures displayed on screens for our wonder and amusement.
The field of cognitive neuroscience is the study of how brain activity is linked with
our mental processes, including thinking, perception, memory, and language. Like other
kinds of neuroscience, it uses neuroimaging technologies to consider links between specific
brain states and conscious experiences. And there’s more than one way to scan a
brain. Structural imaging shows the brain’s anatomy, and is useful in identifying large-scale
tumors, diseases, and injuries. In contrast, functional imaging shows us electromagnetic
or metabolic activity in the brain, like blood flow, to let us observe correlations between
specific mental functions and activity in particular brain areas.
So, yes, neuroimaging has been revolutionizing the field of psychology, much like telescopes
and microscopes did for astronomy and biology. But on the other hand, some of this technology
is very new, and there’s plenty of disagreement about how to interpret neuroimaging findings.
Remember, correlation does not equal causation. So, activity in a certain brain region while
having certain kinds of thoughts might be useful to know, but it’s not the end of
the conversation. We’ve already talked a lot about how function
is often localized in the brain and how everything psychological is simultaneously biological--so
it stands to reason our thoughts and emotions could in part be illustrated by a bright flare
on a dark screen. We’ve also collected a fair amount of evidence
that we don’t just have one layer of consciousness - a single tape playing various tunes - but
rather, something more like two layers, each supported by its own personal bio-psycho-social
pit crew. I’m talking about one of the dual process
models of consciousness--the idea that our conscious, deliberate mind could be saying,
look! a squirrel! while our implicit, automatic mind is simultaneously subprocessing like
a computer: color: brown, tail: bushy, movement: climbing, distance: 20 meters, association:
my sister had a squirrel phobia as a child, implicit bias: I think that squirrels are
ruining America. All of which might weigh upon my behavior upon seeing the little guy.
By some estimates, all your senses are scooping up nearly 11 million bits of information,
EVERY SECOND. And yet, you consciously register only about 40 at time. So how do we keep focused
and filter out all the chatter to actually get stuff done?
With selective attention, of course! Selective attention is how we focus our consciousness
on one particular stimulus or group of stimuli, effectively tuning out the rest. Your consciousness
is like a spotlight on a busy stage. There are other things going on around you that
your automatic, subprocessor brain is covertly registering. But for those moments when you
shine your spotlight, most of the other stimuli fall away.
Try it at home! Right now, you’re consciously watching this lesson on consciousness. You
probably don’t notice the feel of your socks on your feet, or the tongue that’s inside
your mouth, always filling up your mouth with tongue! But as soon as I mention it, the spotlight
of your attention turns to them, you feel those socks on your feet, and you’re like
wow! It’s weird that there’s a tongue in my mouth!
The classic auditory example of selective attention is the cocktail party effect. You
could be in a room with 47 people jabbering away, and yet be able to concentrate your
hearing on one conversation, tuning out the rest of the voices and background music. But,
if the couple next to you were to speak your name, suddenly your cognitive radar would
light up and your attention would whip around to the sound of your name, probably trying
to figure out if Bernice was talking behind your back again.
Bernice!!
This roving spotlight of selective attention is pretty handy most of the time, for spies
and laypeople alike. But it can also be dangerous, if you’re being dumb, and say, texting and
driving. When you shift your primary selective attention
from driving to OMG, LOLOLOLOLOL, you also unwittingly activate your selective inattention,
which means that you failed to see that cyclist who you almost ran over, which would not only
have ruined her life but also yours so DON’T TEXT AND DRIVE!
In fact, when your full attention is directed elsewhere, you’d be astounded by the scope
of obvious things you fail to notice. It’s called inattentional blindness.
You may have even already been subject to one of the most famous experiments of inattentional
blindness...the Invisible Gorilla or, sometimes, the Moonwalking bear. Just google either of
those things if you want to be tested on your awareness and then come back.
Pretty great, right?! Given the prompt to count the number of passes one team makes,
your consciousness is focused on following the players and the ball, nothing else. You
don’t see the players in black, they’re the distraction... also you certainly don’t
see the dancing gorilla...or bear...whichever one.
The original version of this experiment found that about 50% of people didn’t notice that
there was A GORILLA WALKING THROUGH THE ROOM!
THAT is how powerfully selective our attention can be. Something to remember next time you’re
behind the wheel.
But you know who understands and exploits inattentional blindness better than anyone?
Magicians! Except they call it misdirection. Famous modern
magician Teller, of Penn and Teller, says “Every time you perform a magic trick you’re
engaging in a experimental psychology.” And we can’t help but be rubes.
Magicians also prey on our change blindness, the psychological phenomenon in which we fail
to notice changes in our environment. And no, I don’t mean climate change. I mean
the failure to recognize the difference between what was there a moment ago, versus what is
there now. For example, I have changed shirts several
times since this lesson started.
In a well-known and often-copied experiment, sometimes called the “person swap,” an
experimenter will stop someone in a park and ask for directions. And then, during some
staged interruption, the original experimenter will leave and be replaced with a totally
different person. Half the time, the subject doesn’t even notice.
Fun!
One of the many perks of studying psychology with me is you learn all kinds of new ways
to mess with people!
But while change blindness makes for some really cool parlor tricks, this failure to
notice certain things can be dangerous -- say, faulty memories lead to false eyewitness testimonies
in court, or when friends get deadlocked in a he-said, she-said disagreement.
So, my friends, use The Force. But use it wisely. As one of my favorite psychologists
once advised: “A Jedi uses the Force for knowledge and defense, never for attack.”
Actually, that was Yoda. Anyway, the bottom line is, we are far less
aware of what’s going on around us than we think we are. And that’s just when we’re
awake! Imagine what might slip your notice when you’re half-asleep, drunk, hypnotized,
or hallucinating! That’s what we’re gonna talk about next time.
Thanks for watching this episode of Crash Course, if you were selectively conscious
of my words, you got introduced the our constant struggle to define consciousness, learned
a little bit about neuroimaging and its developing role in psychological science, how our consciousness
is split into two pieces, deliberate and automatic, and how the brain can be selectively attentive,
selectively inattentive, and blind to changes in some surprisingly major ways.
If you’d like to sponsor an episode of Crash Course Psychology, get a copy of one of our
Rorschach prints, and even be animated into an upcoming episode, just go to subbable.com/crashcourse,
and subscribe. This episode was written by Kathleen Yale,
edited by Blake de Pastino, and our consultant, is Dr. Ranjit Bhagwat. Our director and editor
is Nicholas Jenkins, the script supervisor is Michael Aranda who’s also our sound designer,
and our graphics team is Thought Café.