Placeholder Image

字幕表 動画を再生する

  • Professor Paul Bloom: So, most of what we do these

  • daysour methods, our theories,

  • our ideasare shaped, to some extent,

  • by Piaget's influence. And so, what I want to do is

  • begin this class that's going to talk about cognitive development

  • by talking about his ideas. His idea was that children are

  • active thinkers; they're trying to figure out

  • the world. He often described them as

  • little scientists. And incidentally,

  • to know where he's coming from on this, he had a very dramatic

  • and ambitious goal. He didn't start off because he

  • was interested in children. He started off because he was

  • interested in the emergence of knowledge in general.

  • It was a discipline he described as genetic

  • epistemologythe origins of knowledge.

  • But he studied development of the individual child because he

  • was convinced that this development will tell him about

  • the development of knowledge more generally.

  • There's a very snooty phrase that--I don't know if you ever

  • heard it before. It's a great phrase.

  • It's "Ontogeny recapitulates phylogeny."

  • And the idea of this--What that means is that development of an

  • individual mimics or repeats development of the species.

  • Now, it's entirely not true, but it's a beautiful phrase and

  • Piaget was committed to this. He was very interested in

  • saying, "Look. We'll figure how a kid develops

  • and that will tell us about the development of knowledge more

  • generally." So, Piaget viewed the child as

  • a scientist who developed this understanding,

  • these schemas, these little,

  • miniature theories of the world.

  • And they did this through two sorts of mechanisms:

  • assimilation and accommodation. So, assimilation would be the

  • act of expanding the range of things that you respond to.

  • Piaget's example would be a baby who's used to sucking on a

  • breast might come to suck on a bottle or on a rattle.

  • That's changing the scope of things that you respond to.

  • Accommodation is changing how you do it.

  • A baby will form his mouth differently depending on what

  • he's sucking on. And so, these processes where

  • you take in--I'm giving this in a very physical way,

  • but in a more psychological sense you have a way of looking

  • at the world. You could expand it to

  • encompass new things, assimilation.

  • But you could also change your system of knowledge itself

  • accommodation. And Piaget argued that these

  • two mechanisms of learning drove the child through different

  • stages. And he had a stage theory,

  • which was quite different from the Freudian stage theory that

  • we have been introduced to. So his methods were to ask

  • children to solve problems and to ask them questions.

  • And his discoveries that--they did them in different ways at

  • different ages led to the emergence of the Stage Theory.

  • So, for Piaget, the first stage is the

  • sensorimotor stage or the sensorimotor period.

  • For here the child is purely a physical creature.

  • The child has no understanding in any real way of the external

  • world. There's no understanding of the

  • past, no understanding of the future, no stability,

  • no differentiation. The child just touches and

  • sees, but doesn't yet reason.

  • And it's through this stage that a child gradually comes to

  • acquire object permanence.

  • Object permanence is the understanding that things exist

  • when you no longer see them. So those of you in front,

  • you're looking at me and I go. It occurred to me it'd be a

  • great magic trick if I then appeared in back.

  • But no, I'm just here. That's object permanence.

  • If I went under here and then the people said,

  • "Where the hell did he go? Class is over," that would show

  • a lack of object permanence. So, adults have object

  • permanence. Piaget's very interesting claim

  • is that kids don't. Before six-month-olds,

  • Piaget observed, you take an object the kid

  • likes like a rattle, you hide it,

  • you put it behind something, it's like it's gone.

  • And he claimed the child really thinks it's just gone.

  • Things don't continue to exist when I'm not looking at them

  • anymore. And so he noticed they--they're

  • surprised by peek-a-boo. And Piaget's claim was one

  • reason why they're surprised at peek-a-boo is you go--you look

  • at a kid, the kid's smiling and go,

  • "Oh, peek-a-boo," and you close--and you cover your face

  • and the kid says, "He's gone."

  • "Peek-a-boo." "Oh, there he is.

  • He's gone." And you really--That's the

  • claim. Piaget also discovered that

  • older children fail at a task that's known as the A-not-B

  • task. And Peter Gray in his

  • psychology textbook refers to it as the "changing hiding places"

  • problem, which is probably a better name for it.

  • And here's the idea. You take a nine-month-old and

  • for Piaget a nine-month-old is just starting to make sense of

  • objects and their permanence. You take an object and you put

  • it here in a cup where the kid can't see it,

  • but it's in the cup. So the kid, if you were the

  • kid, will reach for it. You do it again, reach for it.

  • You do it again, reach for it. That's point A.

  • Then you take--you move it over here.

  • Piaget observed kids would still reach for this.

  • It's like they're not smart enough to figure out that it's

  • not there anymore, even if they see it move.

  • And this was more evidence that they just don't understand

  • objects, and that this thing takes a lot of time and learning

  • to develop. The next stage is the

  • preoperational stage. The child starts off grasping

  • the world only in a physical way, in a sensorimotor way,

  • but when he gets to the preoperational period the

  • capacity to represent the world, to have the world inside your

  • head, comes into being. But it's limited and it's

  • limited in a couple of striking ways.

  • One way in which it's limited is that children are egocentric.

  • Now, egocentrism has a meaning in common English which means to

  • be selfish. Piaget meant it in a more

  • technical way. He claimed that children at

  • this age literally can't understand that others can see

  • the world differently from them. So, one of his demonstrations

  • was the three mountains task. We have three mountains over

  • there. You put a child on one side of

  • the mountains and you ask him to draw it, and a four- or

  • five-year-old can do it easily, but then you ask him to draw it

  • as it would appear from the other side and children find

  • this extraordinarily difficult. They find it very difficult to

  • grasp the world as another person might see it.

  • Another significant finding Piaget had about this phase of

  • development concerns what's called "conservation."

  • The notion of conservation is that there's ways to transform

  • things such that some aspects of them change but others remain

  • the same. So, for instance,

  • if you take a glass of water and you pour it into another

  • glass that's shallow or tall, it won't change the amount of

  • water you have. If you take a bunch of pennies

  • and you spread them out, you don't get more pennies.

  • But kids, according to Piaget, don't know that and this is one

  • of the real cool demonstrations. Any of you who have access to a

  • four- or five-year-old, [laughter]

  • a sibling or something--Do not take one without permission,

  • but if you have access to a four- or five-year-old you can

  • do this yourself. This is what it looks like.

  • The first one has no sound. The second one is going to be

  • sound that's going to come on at the end.

  • But there's two rows of checkers.

  • She asks the kid which one has more.

  • The kid says they're the same. Then she says--Now she asks him

  • which one has more, that or that.

  • So that's really stupid. And it's an amazing finding

  • kids will do that and it's a robust finding.

  • Here's another example. So, they're the same.

  • So, it's a cool finding of that stage, suggesting a limitation

  • in how you deal and make sense of the world.

  • The next phase, concrete operations,

  • from seven to twelve, you can solve the conservation

  • problem, but still you're limited to the

  • extent you're capable of abstract reasoning.

  • So the mathematical notions of infinity or logical notions like

  • logical entailment are beyond a child of this age.

  • The child is able to do a lot, but still it's to some extent

  • stuck in the concrete world. And then finally,

  • at around age twelve, you could get abstract and

  • scientific reasoning. And this is the Piagetian

  • theory in very brief form. Now, Piaget fared a lot better

  • than did Freud or Skinner for several reasons.

  • One reason is these are interesting and falsifiable

  • claims about child development. So claims that--about the

  • failure of conservation in children at different ages could

  • be easily tested and systematically tested,

  • and in fact, there's a lot of support for

  • them. Piaget had a rich theoretical