字幕表 動画を再生する
- So here's a straight forward question.
What color are the strawberries in this photograph?
They're red, right?
Wrong.
Those strawberries are gray.
If you don't believe me, we look for one of the reddest
looking patches on this image, cut it out.
Now what color is that?
[upbeat music]
It's gray, right?
But when you put it back on the image,
it's red again.
It's weird, right?
This illusion was created by a Japanese researcher
named Akiyoshi Kitaoka,
and it hinges on something called color constancy.
It's an incredible visual phenomenon by which the color
of an object appears to stay more or less the same
regardless of the lighting conditions
under which you see it,
or the lighting conditions under which your brain
thinks you're seeing it.
To explain how color constancy works,
we're gonna be looking at a whole bunch of visual illusions
that mess with the way you perceive color.
Illusions like this one.
Can you tell which of the squares on the left
is the same color as a square on the right?
It's probably not the ones you think.
To help us out, we called up David Eagleman.
He's a neuroscientist at Stanford
and an expert in visual illusions.
You might remember him from our previous episode.
That looks so cool!
[David laughs]
We invited him to WIRED's offices in San Francisco
to spend the day running some experiments.
What is color constancy?
- The brain wants to see on object
as a particular color all the time,
irrespective of what the lighting condition is.
- [Host] Light has a lot to do with how you perceive color,
in large part because light itself can be color.
Tungsten light, named for the filament inside
of incandescent bulbs is orange.
And the color of daylight can vary dramatically
from blueish white at midday
to vibrant reds and yellows and oranges at sunset.
- So for example, we were just outside,
and I was holding white a coffee cup,
and it looks white to you.
And now inside with totally different lighting conditions,
it still looks white to you
even though what's actually hitting your eyes is different
in the lighting.
Because of what's called the illuminate
that comes and reflects off of this,
what exactly hits you is very different in these cases.
This would look white to you if we were under tungsten
or fluorescent or an incandescent bulb.
That's what color constancy is.
- So what's going on with
that picture of the strawberries?
To find out, we worked with an artist named Reina Takahashi
to create some paper strawberries
and put them under different lights.
This looks kind of like midday sun.
It's this very, very bright, clear, kind of white color.
- Yep.
- And this is a much more yellow.
This is clearly like a tungsten light.
So what happens if we move these into the lights?
- Let's try it.
Okay.
They do look slightly different,
but they remain red even though
what's called the illuminate is quite different on them.
- Now there's something interesting going on here.
You can rationally recognize that these objects
are different colors, but your brain still classifies
both grouping of strawberries as red,
which brings us back to Kitaoka's photo.
You might've noticed it has a kind of blue-green
overlay to it.
Researchers think that your visual system
perceives that overlay as the color of the light
that's hitting the strawberries.
And it corrects for that light by subtracting it
from the actual physical gray color of the pixels
in the image.
This causes you to perceive the berries as red.
So check this out.
We have some filters here that like the overlay
on the strawberry photo is blue-green,
which is opposite red on the color spectrum.
And that means if you use these filters to cover up
our camera lens, it actually blocks red light.
So if we now point our camera at these red objects,
the pixels on your computer screen are technically gray.
But to you, these objects probably still look red.
- That's what color constancy is,
is the brain always trying to say
what is that object actually in the world?
- So with that said, you might think that the reason
these strawberries look red to you is because you know
strawberries are supposed to be red.
And while researchers think that might be part
of why this illusions is so compelling,
it's not the whole story.
And here's how we know that.
These objects we just showed you,
unlike with strawberries, you have no prior memory
as to what color this kind of object should or shouldn't be.
And yet when we filter our lens to block the red light,
they still look red to you,
which when you think about it
is a pretty amazing feature of human vision and the brain.
Except there are also illusions that can leverage
that very feature against you.
Okay.
So this is the painting.
- Ah. - It's by an artist
named James Gurney.
You've seen this. - Yeah.
Not that painting, but this sort of illusion, yeah.
- [Host] Okay, so you're familiar with the conceit.
- My guess is that even though it looks like
this is under green light and that's under red light,
that the physical paint in one of these squares,
one of these squares is the same and yet they look
totally different these two conditions.
- Right, exactly.
And it's interesting you say light
'cause that's the condition we're just coming from.
Identical object under slightly different light.
And that is by appearances what looks like
what's going on here.
You've got an identical cube under what looks like
a kind of greenish light and under a kind of reddish light.
But in fact, it's actually entirely different
colors of paint.
But so the brain, maybe it doesn't matter entirely?
- That's exactly, it doesn't matter.
Because it's just what hits your retina.
And usually what hits your retina,
your brain tries to figure out
what is the illuminate that's hitting that and reflecting.
But it doesn't matter.
You can just cheat it.
So at the end it's all coming off
and hitting your retina this way.
- Right.
To help us illustrate what's going on here,
we wanted to bring the painting into the real world
and make it human sized.
So our team built this giant version out of paper.
Okay, so we've done our best to reproduce
Gurney's painting in the real world.
How's that, Juno?
- It's good. - Good?
- Instead of using paint,
what we've used is construction paper.
And interestingly one of the pieces
of construction paper there is exactly the same color
as one of the pieces here.
- Okay, and so when I look at this from where I'm standing,
this upper right cube looks the same color
as that upper right cube.
And this lower right cube looks the same color
as that lower right cube.
But that's not the match.
- Yeah, so the matching squares are actually
this one on the lower right.
- And this one on the top here.
So let's go ahead and prove that,
which we can't do in the painting,
but we can do physically like this.
Okay.
So you can see
that this is exactly the same color
of construction paper here.
- [laughs] And that is,
yeah, that is 100% the same piece of paper.
- [laughs] So if we put it back over there,
the key is that because this side of the world
appears to be bathed in a green illuminate
and this is in a red illuminate,
they end up looking quite different
because the way your brain judges the color
has to do with all the surrounding colors
as well as the illuminate around.
And so what your brain serves up to you
can be completely different that what is actually
physically hitting your retina.
- Okay, so here is my question.
Is this happening in my eye?
Or is it happening in my brain?
- Ah, good question.
That has been debated in the literature
since the beginning of these sorts of illusions.
And the answer is it's both.
There's lots of stuff happening at the level of the eye
all the way back to your brain making what are called
unconscious inferences about the world.
In other words, it's guesses about the world
based on its prior assumptions.
And so there's things happening
at all these different levels.
Your brain takes into account the context all around
and then serves up some story about what it thinks
the color is based on what is most useful.
- Right, 'cause if your brain was a complete literalist,
it would have no problem telling that that square
and that square are the exact same color.
- Yeah, that's right.
- Okay, so for me, this raises the inevitable question
that every college student has, you know, postulated
in their dorm room, which is like, how do I know
that the color I see is the same color you see?
- Right.
Actually you can't know that.
We don't know that.
Your mother taught you call this green,
and my mother taught me to call this green.
So we can transact and negotiate in the outside world,
and I can say pass the green thing.
And you can do it.
But our internal experience, we don't know if it's
the same thing on the inside or not.
And in fact, one of the things that's been surfacing lately
on the internet a lot are illusions where we can actually
demonstrate that people are having slightly different
perceptions of what's going on.
- Like the dress.
- Like the dress!
- [Host] You remember the dress, right?
Of course, you remember the dress.
Everybody remembers the dress, the viral internet sensation
that divided the internet back in 2015.
Some people saw it as blue and black.
Other people saw it as white and gold.
Still drives people up the wall.
So which camp is right?
Well, the short answer is neither.
The actual pixels in the image are blue and brown.
But the full answer is a bit more complicated.
Scientists still aren't sure why two people
can see the dress so differently.
But a popular hypothesis is that the colors you see
depend on how your brain interprets
the light hitting the dress.
If your brain thinks the light falling on the dress
is blue, it subtracts that color from the pixels
in the image, and you're more likely to see gold and white.
Conversely, if your brain thinks the light
falling on the dress is more yellow in hue,
it subtracts yellow from the pixels,
and you're more likely to see blue and black.
In other words, the colors you see depend on
whether you attribute the blue in the image to the dress
or the light falling on it.
And believe it or not, this isn't the only time
the internet has been divided over the color
of a piece of clothing.
A couple of years after the dress, this photo of a shoe
went viral for the same reason.
Some people see it as gray and teal,
while others see it as pink and white.
Okay, so then I feel like the really big question then is
do you think it would be possible for us
to physically reproduce one of those ambiguous illusions?
- I think we could tweak the lighting.
The difficulty is knowing.
We'll see it in just one way.
And the question is.
- [laughs] How do you test it?
- If we don't see it often in ways,
how do we know if captured?
Well, we'll have to find some other people,
and ask a bunch of people.
And if some people say it's one way
and some people say the other way,
then we've reproduced it.
- Right.
- Yeah.
Well, let's give it a shot.
- All right, let's try it.
- Okay, good, all right.
- So we set about applying everything we learned
up to that point.
We knew that our brains would try to correct
for the color of light hitting the shoes.
And we also hypothesized that the more ambiguous
that lighting was, the more ambiguous the color
of the shoe would ultimately be.
We tried using gels and different kinds of lights,
and we tried mixing up what was
in the background of the image,
all to make it harder for our brains to determine
what color light was falling on the shoe.
But we couldn't quite get the colors right
straight out of the camera.
So Juno took our picture into Photoshop
to shift the hues around, and it worked,
at least, it did for us.
So to see if lots of other people would see it differently,
we asked folks around the office.
- Yeah, gray and teal.
- It's pink and white, obviously.
- Gray and teal, definitely.
- It's pink and white.
- It's pink and white.
- Those are gray and sea-foam green.
- Pink and white.
- I think it's gray and teal.
- What? - Mm-hmm.
- No. - Yeah.
- [Host] Remember we called this an ambiguous illusion?
Well, that's partly because different people
see it differently, but it's also in part
because for some people it switches back and forth.
- Wait, can I actually?
Well, now it looks kind of pink and white.
Hold on.
Is this like? - You're looking away?
- Say again. - We turned true tone off.
- Okay, so now it looks pink and white to me.
- I see pink so the color changed
in the last like 20, like 15 seconds?
In a sense, Shay, it did.
- [Host] If you don't believe us, we posted some links
in the show notes below that might help you
see the other colors.
The fact is there is a lot we don't understand about color,
which might seem strange because color is such a fundamental
part of our lives.
But it wasn't just the internet that flipped out
over the dress and the shoe.
It was scientists too.
Before the dress, researchers had never observed
such stark differences of opinion over the color
of an object.
And they're still not entirely sure
why those differences exist.
- Everybody's brain is a little bit different
so if you come to the table with different expectations
about what you're seeing, you'll see it differently.
- [Host] One of the theories about the dress is that
how you see it might have to do with
whether you're an earl riser or a night owl.
The idea is that people who stay up late
are more likely to perceive the dress as blue
because they spend more time seeing things
in artificial yellow light.
Whereas early risers are more likely to perceive
the dress as white and gold because they spend more time
observing objects under natural blueish light.
Color adds this layer of information to your perception
of the world, allowing you to distinguish between objects
and react quickly in a variety of situations.
Most of the time, you don't even think about
all the work your visual system does
to serve you that information,
but every once in a while, an illusion like the dress
or the shoes or Kitaoka strawberries comes along
to remind you of all the things
your brain takes care of under the hood,
which is pretty amazing, right?
[playful string music]