Placeholder Image

字幕表 動画を再生する

  • Professor Paul Bloom: I'll begin the class officially

  • with a different sort of demonstration.

  • I want to just show you one of the change-blindness studies

  • that has been done in the real world.

  • And these videotapes are not available publicly.

  • We get them from the web and see them as little Java scripts.

  • So, this is one of the first studies done by Dan Simons when

  • he was at Cornell. And his adviser at the time was

  • our Frank Keil, who's now in our department.

  • So, here's the study.

  • [laughter] And you don't notice it.

  • Change blindness is one of the more striking phenomena

  • discovered by laboratory scientists and by psychologists.

  • But it's important to realize, to get away from the sort of

  • surprise of the gorilla and the fact that it's hard to see the

  • flickering--the object that's flickering,

  • and appreciate the big moral of this, because the big moral of

  • this is actually, I think, striking and quite

  • important. You think right now that you're

  • perceiving the world. I look down on you and I think

  • I have a whole sense of where everybody is.

  • I can't see everybody perfectly in back.

  • You're kind of far away and blurry but there's a sense in

  • which I have a world around me. Similarly, if I'm to close my

  • eyes for a second, everything just remains and I

  • could sort of remember some of the things that are there.

  • That's really good sound localization by me .

  • So you're looking up and you think you have a sense of the

  • world both in perception and memory.

  • The change-blindness experiment suggested this isn't true.

  • The change-blindness experiment suggests that if you look at me

  • for a second and during that second all of your classmates

  • change positions, including those next to you,

  • you are extremely unlikely to notice.

  • The change-blindness experiment suggests that if you turn your

  • eyes away from me towards there for a second and turn back,

  • and I'm dressed entirely differently, you wouldn't

  • notice. The exceptions would be if you

  • told yourself consciously, "Remember what this guy is

  • wearing; he's wearing this,

  • that and the other." But if you don't do it

  • consciously you'll lose it, and usually this is okay.

  • Usually, it's okay because your memory and your visual system

  • exploits a basic fact about the universe,

  • which is that most things stay the same most of the time.

  • I don't have to explicitly remember that you're over there

  • when I turn my head for a second because you'll be over there in

  • any case. You don't need to hold precise

  • representations of the world. And so you only notice it in

  • certain clever circumstances. One sort of clever circumstance

  • is when psychologists change reality as in the

  • change-blindness studies. A second sort of circumstance

  • is in movies. So, one of the big surprises

  • when people started making movies involving cuts was it is

  • extremely difficult to get everything continuously right.

  • And you need to work very hard to notice.

  • So, there's all of these continuity errors that creep up

  • into movies and you have to be a film buff or writing it down to

  • even notice this. And the overall moral here then

  • is that your perception of reality is a lot more sparse,

  • a lot more limited, than you might think it is.

  • So, this is where we were at the end of last class.

  • We were talking about the different sorts of memories:

  • Sensory memory, which is the sort of fraction

  • of a second of sensory residue of what you're hearing and what

  • you're seeing, working memory,

  • short-term memory, and then long-term memory.

  • And we talked last class about how things get into sensory

  • memory, into working memory, the role of attention.

  • And in fact, the change-blindness studies

  • are actually just studies of how something gets from your senses

  • to your consciousness and what does and what doesn't.

  • Now I want to move to the distinction between working

  • memory, short-term memory, and long-term memory.

  • Now, the obvious distinction is actually just in fact--is

  • storage differences. So, long-term memory or "LTM"

  • has a huge storage capacity. This is your memory like the

  • hard drive of your computer. This is the memory you walk

  • around with. It includes all the words in

  • English, just for example, 60 to 80,000 words.

  • It includes everybody you've ever met, languages,

  • faces, stories, locations, nursery rhymes,

  • songs, TV programs. Nobody knows the storage.

  • It is not true that you remember everything that has

  • ever happened to you. There's no reason to believe

  • that this is true. At the same time though,

  • you have a huge amount stored in your brain in long-term

  • storage and nobody actually--It has to be limited because it's a

  • finite, limited brain.

  • But nobody knows how big it is. Nobody knows how many terabytes

  • you carry around in your brain and--but it's a lot.

  • Compare this to working memorythe short-term memory,

  • which is actually very limited. Your memory of what you could

  • store on--in--where you could hold in consciousness right now

  • is quite limited. Here is an exercise.

  • Do not write these things down. I want you to remember them.

  • I'm just going to give you a few numbers: 14,59,

  • 11,109, 43,58, 98,487, 25,389,

  • 54. Please write them down.

  • View this as an IQ test if that would relax you.

  • How many of you who decided to participate in this experiment

  • got three or less? Good.

  • Good. Four, five, six,

  • seven, eight, nine or more?

  • Anybody get all eleven? This is a particularly

  • difficult memory task. The numbers are meaningless.

  • And I told--and I forgot to tell you to get your pen and

  • pencil ready, so some of you just glared at

  • me. But [laughter]

  • under normal circumstances the cognitive psychologist George

  • Miller said that this sort of suggested that the standard

  • memory storage of short-term memory is seven,

  • plus or minus two. And what that means is anywhere

  • from five to nine roughly. Some of you,

  • I bet, can beat that. Some of you on a not-so-good

  • day maybe won't make it that much.

  • Now "seven plus or minus two" is what you--;so,

  • that's what you hold in consciousness.

  • I can tell you 14,21. You walk around,

  • "Oh, yeah, 14,21." You hold that in consciousness

  • with no problem. But I throw eleven numbers at

  • you, you can't. Some dribble out.

  • You can't hold that in your conscious window in your

  • short-term memory. Now, this raises the question

  • "seven plus or minus two" what? And the answer seems to be what

  • George Miller calls "chunks." And a chunk is a basic memory

  • unit, something you think of as a single, individual entity.

  • So, suppose you see the string of letters "L,

  • A, M, A, I, S, O, N."

  • If you don't know--If you can't form these into words and you

  • have to remember them, these are eight chunks.

  • You have to just pick them up separately.

  • On the other hand, if you break them up into four

  • words you could just remember it as four chunks.

  • And if you break it up into two words in French,

  • "la maison," "the house," it could just be one or two.

  • How much you know depends--affects how much you

  • memorize--how much you could store in memory because it

  • affects what counts as a basic unit of memory.

  • And there's all sorts of examples of this.

  • If I tell you "1,1, 0,1, 1,0, 0,1,

  • 0,1, 1,0," those of you who don't know binary numbers might

  • have to remember that as "1, 1,0, 0," whatever I said.

  • Those of you who are computer scientists or mathematicians or,

  • for whatever reason, know binary numbers could

  • convert it into a single binary number.

  • Anybody know what the number is? No, I cannot say it again.

  • [laughter] Some number,

  • 24, or not 24--to some number, 24, and then you remember "24."

  • It's easier. Suppose you see a chessboard

  • and the chessboard is set up and you don't know how to play

  • chess. It is murderously hard to

  • remember that. They've done the experiments.

  • They've taken people in a lab who don't know how to play

  • chess. They set up a chessboard and

  • then they say, "Okay.

  • Look at this for five minutes." Then they take it away,

  • set it up again, and it's murderously hard.

  • "There is a horse-y thing on the side there and everything."

  • But if these chess pieces are set up in some way that's

  • logical for a chess player, then a chess master could look

  • at it and remember it in a glance, "Oh.

  • It's the Fibonacci defense" or something like that [laughs],

  • and then immediately recover it.

  • Similarly, football coaches have been tested on their

  • memories of football diagrams. And they have a photographic

  • memory for football diagrams because it corresponds to things

  • that make sense. Architects could have a

  • photographic memory, a perfect memory for floor

  • plans because it makes sense to them.

  • They understand it. And so the way you store things

  • in memory, and this is a theme we're going to return to when we

  • get to long-term memory, depends in a large extent on

  • how much you understand it. And this shows up in expertise

  • effects. Now, this is what's happening

  • so far in short-term memory, how much you hold in there.

  • The question is how do you get it into long-term memory?

  • So, you have long-term memory, your major storage system.

  • How does information get from your consciousness to long-term

  • storage? Well, there's one

  • thing--there's one way which sort of works sometimes but not

  • very well. And it's called "maintenance

  • rehearsal." Suppose I said you have to

  • remember this number, this string of numbers.

  • And if you remember it in twenty minutes you will get one

  • thousand dollars. And the string is my phone

  • number when I was a kid. I'll include the area code:

  • 514-688-9057. Now, if you tell that to a

  • four-year-old, well, the four-year-old will

  • say, "I'll remember it." And then you ask them,

  • "What did I just say?" "Well, I don't know."

  • If you tell it to an--because you know something--If a lot

  • depended on it, you would know to do something.

  • What you would do is you'd say to yourself, "514-688-9057,51

  • 4-688-9057,514-688 --" You'd rehearse it in your head over

  • and over again. The problem is you could hold

  • it as long as you can do that. It's like these movies.

  • You see this all the time, like an episode of 24:

  • "Jack, call CTU and tell them Agent 11 is trapped in a--" And

  • I can't even remember this but the way to remember it is you

  • hold--you've just got to repeat it over and over again in your

  • head. But this will not typically get

  • things into long-term memory. To get things into long-term

  • memory, rehearsal is usually not enough.

  • You need to do other things. Typically, what you need is

  • structure and organization. And one way to demonstrate this

  • was in a classic "depth of processing" experiment which

  • nicely illustrates the fact that the more you structure

  • something, the deeper you think about it,

  • the better it gets entrenched in the long-term memory.

  • So, in this study what they did was they asked people--they told

  • people that there's going to be words flashed on a screen.

  • And all of the subjects saw the same strings of words.

  • There were forty-eight words. They were not told to memorize

  • the words. One third of the subjects was

  • told, "Look. Some of these words are going

  • to come out in capital letters, some of them not capital

  • letters. Press a button for capitals,

  • non-capitals." "Sure."

  • The other group was told, "Some of these words will rhyme

  • with 'train,' Others won't. Press a button if it rhymes in

  • 'train'." The third group was told,

  • "Does it fit into the sentenceThe girl placed the blank on

  • the table'? Press a button if it does.

  • Press a button if it doesn't." Then they were asked as a

  • surprise, "What words did you see?"

  • And the findings looked like this.

  • When they were asked to focus on just what the word looked

  • like, memory was very poor, the sound better,

  • the meaning better. If you want to remember

  • something, the best way to remember it is to give it

  • meaning, to give it sense. This is illustrated through a

  • very ancient technique, which is that the way to

  • remember things that are otherwise arbitrary is to give

  • them some organization through memory tricks,

  • through vivid imagery or songs or poetry.

  • And there's a lot of examples of this.

  • Do you know how to remember that the hippocampus--There's a

  • part of the brain called the hippocampus.

  • This is the worst memory trick ever but it will stick with you

  • for twenty years. The hippocampus is involved in

  • spatial memory. It's involved in finding your

  • way around. Think to yourself,

  • "The way I find my way around campus is through the

  • hippocampus." And you think,

  • "Well, that's stupid," but you'll never forget now that the

  • hippocampus is in charge of spatial memory.

  • It's going to be all you retain from this course.

  • Memory books on how to remember people's names usually try to

  • exploit this sort of thing when you try to get poetry or

  • dramatic images. So, the memory books always

  • typically involve somebody--like you meet somebody with very

  • spiky hair and they say, "My name is Mr.

  • Fish" and then you remember--you think of their--of

  • a big fish impaled on their hair.

  • And then whenever you see them you remember their name.

  • It only really works for names like "Fish" but [laughter]

  • the idea is you try to generate vivid imagery.

  • When stuck with a situation where you have to remember ten

  • letters, turn it into a song where--or a dirty poem where

  • each of the letters is the first words of it.

  • When having to remember something that seems totally

  • arbitrary, try to figure out a grand and obscene image that

  • will come to mind easily. And this is how--these are one

  • way to get things into memory. At a deep level though,

  • the way to get things into memory, and this applies to this

  • course no less than anything else,

  • is by understanding the--understanding it.

  • I'm going to read you something and I want you to try your best

  • to remember what I tell you. These are not going to be

  • strings of numbers. These are going to be--This is

  • going to be a series of sentences: "A newspaper is

  • better than a magazine. A seashore is a better place

  • than the street. At first it is better to run

  • than to walk. You may have to try several

  • times. It takes some skill but it's

  • easy to learn. Even young children can enjoy

  • it. Once successful,

  • complications are minimal. Birds seldom get too close.

  • Rain, however, soaks in very fast.

  • Too many people doing the same things can also cause problems.

  • One needs lots of room. If there are no complications,

  • it can be very peaceful. Finally, a rock will serve as

  • an anchor. If things break loose from it,

  • however, you will not have a second chance."

  • And here is what I said . This is murderously hard to

  • remember. Now try it.

  • Knowing what this is about, being able to put a context to

  • it helps the memory and helps it come to mind.

  • [laughter] Okay. So, this is about how to get

  • memory--how to get information into your memory.

  • How do you get information out? So, it's exam period.

  • You got the stuff presumably into your head.

  • You have to get it out. You have to retrieve it.

  • There is a court case. You have to figure out--You

  • have to recount the crime that you witnessed.

  • You see somebody and you want to know his or her name.

  • And you heard it; you just have to get it out.

  • Well, how do you do that? Well, there's "retrieval cues."

  • Retrieval cues make sense. Retrieval cues are just things

  • that have been associated with what you--what you're trying to

  • remember. If I have to remember to

  • replace the windows, when I walk in to my living

  • room and see that a window is cracked that will remind me to

  • replace the windows. If I had a lunch date with you

  • and forgot about it, when I see you,

  • "Oh, yeah. We were supposed to get

  • together to have lunch." Retrieval cues bring things

  • back but it's a little bit more complicated than that.

  • There's a more general relationship between encoding

  • and retrieval called the "compatibility principle."

  • And what this means is you're much better to remember

  • something in the context in which you have learned it.

  • And this is also known as "context-dependent memory" and

  • "state-dependent memory." It's illustrated by one of the

  • strangest experiments in the history of psychology where they

  • had people on a boat and then they had them scuba dive

  • underwater. And they taught them things

  • either on the boat or underwater with things that they held up.

  • And then they tested them later. And it turns out that you'll

  • remember it better if you're tested on it in the context in

  • which you learned it. And it might be because then

  • the retrieval cues help bring it back.

  • But it's more general than that. If you have to remember

  • something you learned in this class, you will do better if you

  • try to think about the room in which you learned it in.

  • You will do better on your final exam if you were to take

  • it in this room than if you were to take it in another room

  • because being in this room will bring back the cues.

  • It's not just the environment. People who learn things when

  • they're stoned remember them better--keeping stoned at a sort

  • of a low-level that doesn't disrupt other mental

  • activities--remember them better when they're sort of stoned

  • again [laughter] than if they're non-stoned.

  • Similarly--So, if you study while you drink

  • you should tipple a little bit before coming in to the Final

  • exam. [laughter]

  • It's sort of like the "what happens in Vegas stays in Vegas"

  • sort of result. And so, similarly,

  • it even applies to moods in that if you learn something when

  • depressed you have a slightly better recovery of it when

  • you're in that same mood of depression than when you're

  • elated. And the idea is that part of

  • what memory is--part of what recovering memory is is getting

  • back your original context in which you learned it.

  • "Elaborative rehearsal" and retrieval involves the

  • connections between different things.

  • Elaborative rehearsal is that the more you think about

  • something the easier it is to remember.

  • If you have to think about--If you have to remember something,

  • try to connect it to as many things as possible.

  • Think of an image. Make a joke out of it.

  • Imagine how you would explain it to somebody else.

  • Imagine how the world--what the world would be like if it wasn't

  • so. And the idea is that this sort

  • of thinking about it makes connections in your memory from

  • that thing you have to learn, to other memories.

  • And so it makes it easier to recover.

  • "Elaborative retrieval" refers to a finding that when you want

  • to get something back out of memory people tend to give up

  • too soon. It turns out that there's a lot

  • of stuff that's in your memory but it needs work to extract;

  • it needs various sort of searching strategies.

  • One study asked people who were considerably older than you to

  • remember their high school classmates.

  • And in the first pass people were terrible.

  • Maybe they had a couple of friends they kept in touch with.

  • Otherwise, pretty bad. And this is a good experiment

  • because you could use high school yearbooks to judge

  • whether or not they get it right.

  • But then what you do is you tell the person,

  • "Look. Keep trying.

  • Were you--What sort of--Who was your teacher?

  • What sort of clubs did you belong to?

  • What sort of sports you--did you participate in?

  • How did you get to school? How did you get from school?

  • What did you do during lunch? What did you do during break?"

  • And you keep ask--"Do you know--have any friends whose

  • letter--whose last name began with ‘B,' with ‘C,' with

  • ‘D'?" And you keep pushing and

  • pushing and pushing. And over the span of time

  • things come back. Again, it's not true that you

  • never forget. There is honest to God

  • forgetting but sometimes you think you forget and it's

  • because you haven't looked long enough.

  • There's a real physical notion of searching for the right

  • answer. We've talked about retrieval.

  • Oh. Every class I've given somebody

  • asks either in class or by e-mail what about déjà vu?

  • And déjà vu is a feeling that an event has happened before.

  • So, you're looking at me and I'm lecturing and you say,

  • "I've heard this before. I know this before."

  • You see somebody and say, "I've been in this situation

  • before." This is not evidence for

  • psychic powers, [laughter]

  • which many people say it is, but nobody really knows why

  • this exists. We know, and this is a clue,

  • it's worse with frontal lobe damage.

  • If you get damage to this part of the brain,

  • you get a lot more déjà vu experiences.

  • I asked some experts in memory, including Marcia Johnson who is

  • chair of our department, what the best explanation for

  • déjà vu is. And the answer she gave,

  • the--say one big theory, goes like this.

  • Déjà vu is a feeling that it's happened before.

  • The answer is it has happened before.

  • It's happened half a second ago. And so what happens is

  • sometimes there is a glitch, a disturbance in the force.

  • I don't know. There's a glitch [laughter]

  • and you are talking and then something happens to you and you

  • put it in your memory. But it's as if you don't put

  • the stamp on it of what time and what date.

  • So, you're talking to me and then you store it in memory but

  • you don't store it in memory as happening right now.

  • Then half a second goes by and you're talking to me and you

  • say, "This is strangely familiar."

  • And that's one theory of what goes on in déjà vu.

  • Okay. So far, there's the sort of

  • good newsrememberingbut then there's bad news

  • forgetting. How many people can remember,

  • without looking down at your notes, at least two of the

  • numbers I gave you earlier?

  • How many people can remember at least four?

  • Oh, impressive. If I asked you in an hour,

  • the number would go down. These are sort of statistics in

  • a similar experiment. And this graph illustrates that

  • people forget. Over time, you'll forget.

  • Why do you forget? Why is there forgetting at all?

  • Well, there's different explanations for this.

  • One explanation is your brain's a physical thing,

  • it's a physical piece of meat, and it kind of goes bad.

  • Physical things decay. And so, the memory traces that

  • are laid onto your brain will just decay over time.

  • A second answer is interference. So, remember those numbers?

  • Here's a few more: 114,81, 66,42.

  • Well, the more information that comes in that's similar to the

  • stuff you're trying to remember, it blocks your recovery of

  • original information. So your ability to remember

  • something can be impaired by learning more things which are

  • related to it because they get confused in memory.

  • Finally, and maybe this is most interesting, there are changes

  • in retrieval cues. So, the more time goes by the

  • more the world changes. And if your memory is to some

  • extent dependent on cues bringing it back to life,

  • then the change in retrieval cues can make it more difficult

  • to recall certain things. This leads to a puzzle where

  • there's considerable scientific debate over the case of

  • childhood amnesia. And the case of childhood

  • amnesia is--doesn't refer to when a child gets brain damage

  • and gets amnesia. What it refers to is people

  • have a difficult time recovering very early memories.

  • I want people to just take a second and try to think back on

  • what your first memory is and roughly how old you were.

  • How many people don't think you have a first memory until you

  • were about five years old or older?

  • Okay. How many people think you have

  • the first memory of around age four or younger?

  • How many people think you have the first memory of around age

  • three or younger? Two or younger?

  • How many of you think you have the first memory when you were

  • about one years old or younger? And I'm not asking about past

  • lives but that [laughs] happened last year.

  • How old is your--roughly your first memory do you think?

  • How old? Student:

  • Between one and two. Professor Paul Bloom:

  • Between one and two? Anybody think they could beat

  • that? Same guy?

  • Yeah. Student: One.

  • Professor Paul Bloom: One.

  • [laughter] Anybody else? The literature is unclear on

  • this because it's very difficult to test people's recollections

  • of their first memories. If I'm to ask people about

  • their first memories, they'll often say,

  • "Oh, yeah. I remember I was in this room

  • and there was a crib and I'm goingGa ga,

  • goo goo' [laughter] and I was on the potty.

  • I was walking. I was so cute.

  • I remember it." It's very difficult to tell

  • and, as we'll discuss in some detail, there are a lot of

  • reasons to distrust people when they--not that they're lying but

  • to distrust the accuracy of people's memories.

  • We also know from studies about trauma where people have

  • terrible experiences when they're one or two.

  • Typically, this trauma is not remembered later on.

  • People know of trauma because they're told about it but they

  • don't typically remember it with any accuracy.

  • Even children--older children don't remember back beyond that

  • age. Nobody knows why childhood

  • amnesia occurs. Nobody knows why it's very

  • difficult to recover memories before about the age of three.

  • One theory is that the retrieval cues change radically.

  • I had a friend of mine who's a clinical psychologist and he

  • suggested a new form of therapy where they make these giant

  • tables and chairs and then they bring you in to the office and

  • you're standing there with these giant tables and chairs

  • [laughter] and all these memories of being

  • a baby would come flooding back. [laughter]

  • And he dropped out of the field and-- [laughter]

  • Really, but it's such a cool idea.

  • Some people think language is to blame.

  • So a child, a baby, starts out with no spoken or

  • signed language. Language comes to be learned at

  • around one, two, and three, and it might be that

  • the learning of a language reformats your memory.

  • And once the memory is reformatted it can't go back to

  • the previous state prior to language in the end.

  • It could be neural maturation. It could be that those memory

  • parts of the brains grow around age two or three that just

  • weren't there prior to that. And nobody really knows.

  • It's a fascinating research area why--about memory changes

  • early on. Another case of memory failure

  • is brain damage. And brain damage comes in a

  • couple of flavors. There is retrograde amnesia;

  • "retro" for past. Retrograde amnesia is when you

  • lose some memory of the past. This could be in a case where

  • you get some sort of head trauma and you lose memory of your

  • entire episodic memory. But typically,

  • if you have any sort of serious accident that involves you

  • losing consciousness you'll have a blackout of some period prior

  • to that, say, blow to the head.

  • And the reason for this is as you're having these experiences

  • now they need to kind of get consolidated into your brain.

  • Your brain needs to rewire and catch up to the experiences

  • you're having. A sudden blow to the head will

  • knock you unconscious and then the memories that have happened

  • immediately prior will not get consolidated and they'll be lost

  • forever. Another sort of memory is

  • anterograde amnesia and this was the case of--This happens in

  • Korsakoff's syndrome. It happens to a very famous

  • patient known as H.M. who actually lives in Hartford,

  • Connecticut. And it happened to Clive

  • Wearing, the film you saw last class.

  • And this sort of amnesia is a sort of amnesia where you lose

  • the ability to form new memories.

  • And so you live in a perpetual present, unable to accumulate

  • new memories. But it's actually a little bit

  • more complicated than that. What happens is--And this was

  • an exciting discovery about these patients that led to some

  • real insights about normal memory--What happens is--And

  • this is the brain damage in these cases,

  • the temporal lobe and the hippocampus, very useful for

  • spatial memory you'll know. One discovery made about people

  • who couldn't form new memories is that they could form new

  • memories, but of certain types. So for example,

  • this is a task here involving filling in a star while looking

  • in to a mirror. And if I asked you to do it

  • you'd find it pretty difficult. It's just kind of difficult to

  • do. You'd be clumsy at it.

  • You bring in an amnesic who can't form new memories and you

  • say, "Hey. I want you to try something new.

  • I want you to try this star game."

  • He'd say, "Okay. I've never seen it before but

  • I'll do it." Tries it.

  • Does very badly. You bring him in and over and

  • over again--Each time he does it he starts off by saying,

  • "I've never seen this before. I'll--I'm sure I'll give it a

  • try" but he gets better and better at it.

  • And this is known as implicit memory.

  • The claim is that in these sorts of cases you lose the

  • abilities to form explicit conscious memories that you're

  • aware of, that you understand. But some sorts of memories

  • persist and you are able to form them.

  • This has actually been illustrated in a couple of

  • dramatic movies, one of them a very bad dramatic

  • movie [laughs] where Drew Barrymore loses the

  • ability to form new memories and somehow falls in love with Adam

  • Sandler. [laughs]

  • Definitely don't watch that. But a very good movie called

  • "Memento," which is about a character who loses his ability

  • to form new memories while trying to track down his wife's

  • killer. "Memento" is a movie which is

  • fascinating because it's told backwards.

  • But throughout "Memento" there's another story told

  • forwards. And I like this story because

  • it very dramatically illustrates what does, and what is and is

  • not impaired in cases of severe memory damage.

  • So, I'm going to show you a couple of clips that illustrate

  • the disassociation from "Memento."

  • Those of you who have seen the movie know that this ends up

  • quite tragically for Sammy. I highly recommend the movie.

  • We've dealt right now with two sorts of failures of memory.

  • One is everyday failure of memory when you forget.

  • How many of you remember three or more of the numbers I

  • originally presented? Yeah?

  • Go ahead. Student: [inaudible]

  • Professor Paul Bloom: Fourteen, 59,11.

  • Is that right? [laughs] Fine.

  • [laughter] All right. I'm going to ask you again in a

  • month. [laughter]

  • Well, people are supposed to forget [laughter]

  • and some things will--you will forget.

  • That's normal forgetting. A second case is forgetting due

  • to brain damage. Forgetting due to brain damage

  • is exotic and unusual but it's interesting in that it

  • illustrates some more general themes about how the mind works.

  • Remember one theme of this course is we're going to look at

  • exotic cases like the case of Clive Wearing,

  • not just because they're interesting in their own right

  • but sometimes by looking at the extremes we could learn

  • something about how normal people's normal,

  • intact minds and brains work. The third case of forgetting is

  • more interesting and it actually--Well,

  • I want to do a little trial here.

  • What I want to do is I want to--You to listen to three

  • children describe an event that happened.

  • I want you to come to some--your own guess.

  • Imagine you were a judge, you were a childcare worker,

  • you wanted to see--I want you to be--come to your own guess

  • about who you believe and what you think happened.

  • [inaudible] [laughter]

  • You've heard three children. Who do you believe?

  • Who believes--There's three of them, one, two,

  • three. Who believes the first one?

  • Who believes the second one? Who believes the third one?

  • Sort of an even split.

  • Twenty-three hundred experts were shown these films and asked

  • about the different actions, whether or not the person

  • ripped the book, messed up the bear,

  • tossed the book in the air and, as you could see,

  • the majority thought that he did.

  • This is work done by Steve Ceci who was gracious enough to lend

  • me the film to use for teaching purposes.

  • It turns out the second girl was right.

  • Absolutely nothing happened. [laughter]

  • The teacher said, "There is somebody named Sam

  • Stone who's going to come in." A guy walks in and says,

  • "Hi," walks around and leaves. [laughter]

  • The first and third children had their memories implanted,

  • not through any sort of science fiction means.

  • They had their memories implanted--Well,

  • they had their memories implanted like this.

  • Some of the children would just ask questions.

  • The interviewer, by the way, was herself unaware

  • of what happened so the interviewer was a perfectly

  • naive interviewer. And it turns out if you just

  • interview children and you ask them questions about whether the

  • book was ripped, "Did you see him?

  • Did he really do it?" they don't say anything.

  • They didn't see anything and they won't say anything.

  • Other children were told about Sam Stone.

  • They were told a stereotype about Sam Stonethat he's

  • very clumsy and he tends to rip things and he trips and he

  • breaks things and he spills things.

  • And in fact, the third child mentioned that

  • in passing. He said, "He always does that."

  • Just knowing this about Sam Stone tends to raise the

  • proportion of kids who say, for instance,

  • that he ripped the book. Other children were given

  • suggestions. They were given suggestive

  • questioning. They were a series of leading

  • questions like, "Oh.

  • Sam Stone came in? Did he rip a book while he was

  • there?" And still more children got

  • both. And in fact,

  • the children you saw were from this group.

  • They heard Sam Stone being described as a clumsy fellow and

  • they were given a series of suggestive questionings.

  • In this condition they were given several suggestive

  • questionings over the period of several months.

  • These children, like the first child and the

  • third child, are not lying. They honestly believe that Sam

  • Stone came in and did these things.

  • Also they believe it and they're so convincing in their

  • belief that experts, including police officers and

  • child caseworkers and judges and lawyers, find these children to

  • be extremely believable. And I think they probably find

  • them to be extremely believable because the children are not

  • lying. They really believe they saw

  • what they saw. But these memories were

  • implanted. And Ceci, and many other

  • investigators, study how memories can be

  • implanted in people's minds through suggestion and through

  • leading questions. It turns out that the same sort

  • of experiments and the same sort of research has been done with

  • considerable success in implanting false memories in

  • adults. There are dramatic cases of

  • people remembering terrible crimes and confessing to them

  • when actually, they didn't commit them.

  • And this is not because they are lying.

  • It's not even because they're, in some obvious sense,

  • deranged or schizophrenic or delusional.

  • Rather, they have persuaded themselves, or more often been

  • persuaded by others, that these things have actually

  • happened. Psychologists have studied in

  • the laboratory how one could do this, how one can implant

  • memories in other people. And some things are sort of

  • standard. Suppose I was to tell you a

  • story about a trip I took to the dentist or a visit I took to--or

  • a time when I ate out at a restaurant and I'm to omit

  • certain details. I omit the fact that I paid the

  • bill in a restaurant, let's say or I finished the

  • meal and then I went home. Still, you will tend to fill in

  • the blanks. You'll tend to fill in the

  • blanks with things you know. So, you might remember this

  • later saying, "Okay.

  • He told me he finished eating, paid the bill and left,"

  • because paying the bill is what you do in a restaurant.

  • This is benign enough. You fill in the blanks.

  • You also can integrate suppositions made by others.

  • And the clearest case of this is eyewitness testimony.

  • And the best research on this has been done by Elizabeth

  • Loftus who has done a series of studies,

  • some discussed in the textbook, showing how people's memories

  • can be swayed by leading questions.

  • And it can be extremely subtle. In one experiment,

  • the person was just asked in the course of a series of

  • questions--shown a scene where there's a car accident and asked

  • either, "Did you see a broken

  • headlight?" or "Did you see the broken

  • headlight?" Thethe' presupposes that

  • there was a broken headlight and in fact, the people told--asked,

  • "Did you see the broken headlight?"

  • later on are more likely to remember one.

  • It creates an image and they fill it in.

  • In another study, she would show film segments

  • and then ask, "Did you see the children

  • getting on the school bus?" Now, there was no school bus

  • but people who hear that question later on when asked,

  • "Did you see a school bus in the film?"

  • are more likely to say yes. In another study,

  • she would show people film segments and ask them either,

  • "How fast were the cars going when they hit each other?"

  • or "How fast were the cars going when they smashed into

  • each other?" A week later she'd bring people

  • back in to the laboratory and ask, "Did you see any broken

  • glass?" Those who hear a smash tend to

  • see the broken glass more than those who hear a hit because the

  • question has changed their memory,

  • making it more of a dramatic event.

  • Hypnosis is the clearest case where there's a sort of

  • reconstructive effort led by--led as a result of leading

  • and probing questions. Some of you are readily

  • hypnotizable and you can be hypnotized.

  • And what we would learn about a past event from hypnotizing you

  • will not necessarily then be inaccurate.

  • What hypnotizing does is it makes people very willing to

  • cooperate. Unfortunately,

  • it isn't as if there is a memory storage there where you

  • could just go through and look as in the movies where you just

  • say, "What's the license plate?"

  • The person's hypnotized and then the flashback comes in and

  • then they zoom in on the license plate.

  • Memory doesn't work that way. What happens is--What somebody

  • will do in a hypnotizable state is they'll be very eager to

  • please the hypnotist. And so they'll make stuff up.

  • And people under hypnosis just make stuff up.

  • And they do very enthusiastically and very

  • believably make stuff up. This is particularly the case

  • with hypnotic regression when we ask you to go back to your sixth

  • birthday party, for instance.

  • And what's great as a developmental psychologist is if

  • I ask you to go back to your fourth birthday party and you're

  • hypnotizable you'll be oh, just like a four-year-old

  • except you won't be like a real four-year-old.

  • What you'll be like is an adult's notion of what a

  • four-year-old is supposed to be. In fact, this has happened in

  • the extreme case with hypnotic regression where people claim to

  • speak languages like from ancient Egypt.

  • And linguists love these studies because you don't--of

  • course you don't really sound like you're speaking a language

  • from ancient Egypt. What you sound is like a North

  • American who believes he's speaking a language from ancient

  • Egypt so they're, "nonsense sounds."

  • [laughter] And so what it makes you

  • is--Hypnotism brings out the actor in you.

  • It makes you want to give a persuasive account of what

  • happened. And so hypnotism is just an

  • extreme form of what normally happens in eyewitness testimony.

  • Repressed memories. We could devote a class--We

  • could devote a semester to the very heated debate in the United

  • States mostly about repressed memories.

  • There are many adults who have claimed to have experienced

  • traumatic sexual abuse. In some cases,

  • this is unexceptional from a memory point of view.

  • People know this happened to them.

  • They've always known it happens to them and then they tell

  • people about it. But there's a subset of cases

  • where people have had no memory up to a point of what happened

  • to them. Then they go to a psychologist

  • or a psychiatrist; they undergo questioning,

  • often using hypnotic techniques;

  • and then they recover a memory of past traumatic sexual abuse.

  • And what this is--what makes this so debatable,

  • and there is a debate about this.

  • I don't want to try to preclude it one way or another.

  • What makes this debatable is some psychologists believe that,

  • in at least some cases, these memories are real and

  • they have been repressed through a Freudian mechanismthat

  • they're too terrible to bring to consciousness,

  • and the therapy brings them out into real life.

  • But most psychologists believe that these memories cannot be

  • trusted, that these memories are created through the actions of

  • the therapist. And so, there's actually

  • considerable psychological and legal battles over the veracity

  • of the therapists where women who have claimed to have

  • sexual--be sexually abused, for instance,

  • have pressed criminal charges against their fathers on the

  • basis of false memories. Similarly, people who have been

  • accused of sexual abuse have pressed criminal charges against

  • psychiatrists claiming that these psychiatrists have

  • implanted the memories into their sons and daughters.

  • It is controversial whether memories are ever repressed.

  • What isn't controversial is that, for at least some cases,

  • you can implant false memories in people,

  • not because you're a sinister or evil person but because you

  • really believed something happened.

  • And you talked to them about it and then you caused these

  • memories to come into being. A final case is flashbulb

  • memories. I asked this early in the

  • semester. I'll ask it again.

  • How many of you remember where you were on September 11,2001?

  • Is there anybody who doesn't remember where they were on

  • September 11,2001? It would be interesting.

  • It was a socially relevant event, but here's the problem

  • with these flashbulb memories. Flashbulb memories are the idea

  • that these memories being so vivid, and they are vivid for

  • many of us--exactly where we stood, what happened;

  • well, they can't really be trusted.

  • And here is why not. Because they are such important

  • events, I bet many of you have actually heard the question

  • before, "Where were you on September 11^(th)?"

  • and talked about it. What happens in these

  • conversations is stories change. I have my--I knew where I was

  • on September 11^(th). My wife knew where she was.

  • But I spent as much time listening to her talk about it

  • as I spent time me talking about it.

  • And now maybe my memory is actually of her experience and

  • not mine. It's not--For all of these

  • cases, the temptation you have to resist is saying,

  • "Yes. I know memories can be swayed.

  • I know they could be distorted and everything but,

  • you see, I really am sure that happened."

  • You have to resist that temptation because there are so

  • many cases we know, including the tape of the girls

  • that we just saw, where people are entirely sure

  • things happened. And we know full well that they

  • didn't exist. Being sure is no guarantee that

  • a memory isn't false, reconstructed or even

  • implanted. So, this part of memory has

  • three main morals. There are many types of

  • memories. I talked about short-term

  • memory, long-term memory. I talked about implicit memory

  • and explicit memory. These are sort of separable

  • sort of memories. You could break one while

  • having the other one impaired. Arguably, there are brain

  • systems dedicated to memory for faces, memory for everyday

  • objects, memory for spatial locations.

  • The key to remembering is organization and understanding.

  • Introduction to "X" courses, including Introduction to

  • Psychology courses, are among the hardest courses

  • at Yale. And the reason why is there is

  • just a lot of material that is diverse and you have to command

  • each aspect separately. The easiest courses at Yale

  • tend to be highfalutin seminars where you kind of have enough of

  • a background that everything is--can be clear and

  • understandable. The more you understand

  • something, the easier you'll remember it.

  • And finally, you can't trust some of your

  • memories. Your reading response for this

  • week is you have to use your powers for good and not for

  • evil, [laughter]

  • though if you manage to succeed at this I will be very

  • impressed. [laughter]

  • But you have to describe, based on the lecture materials

  • and the readings, how to implant a false memory.

  • We have a few minutes. Any questions on memory.

  • Yes. Student: [inaudible]

  • Professor Paul Bloom: Uh huh.

  • Hey. Please-- Student:

  • [inaudible] Is that long-term sensory

  • memory? Professor Paul Bloom:

  • The example is, "What sort of memory is it when

  • you know how to play the piano?" And it's a very good question.

  • It is long-term memory because you might know how to do a

  • concerto or a song and then you have it stored in your head and

  • you carry it around with you. You'll remember it a year from

  • now, two years from now. It is long-term memory but it

  • is also an excellent example of implicit memory because you know

  • how to do it but you could do it unconsciously without attending

  • to it. It's not sensory but it's as

  • if, put it crudely, that your fingers know and not

  • your mind. We have time for one more

  • question. Yes.

  • Student: [inaudible] Professor Paul Bloom:

  • The question is about photographic memory.

  • There are a lot of claims about photographic memory.

  • My understanding is they do not tend to be substantiated.

  • Sometimes photographic memory, and this came up when we talked

  • about autism a few classes ago, is linked with savant-like

  • skills. People who have severe

  • impairments in some ways may have photographic memories in

  • others. I am not convinced that

  • photographic memory in the sense that you see something,

  • you take a picture of it, you hold it in memory really

  • exists. I think there may be one or two

  • case studies that suggest it might be real but I think it's

  • controversial. Okay.

  • We have a guest lecturer on Wednesday.

  • Dean Peter Salovey will talk to us about love.

Professor Paul Bloom: I'll begin the class officially

字幕と単語

ワンタップで英和辞典検索 単語をクリックすると、意味が表示されます

B1 中級

8.現在を意識すること、過去を意識すること。 (8. Conscious of the Present; Conscious of the Past:)

  • 98 25
    CH に公開 2021 年 01 月 14 日
動画の中の単語