上級 46766 タグ追加 保存
動画の字幕をクリックしてすぐ単語の意味を調べられます!
単語帳読み込み中…
字幕の修正報告
We are going to take a quick voyage
over the cognitive history of the 20th century,
because during that century,
our minds have altered dramatically.
As you all know, the cars that people drove in 1900
have altered because the roads are better
and because of technology.
And our minds have altered, too.
We've gone from people who confronted a concrete world
and analyzed that world primarily in terms
of how much it would benefit them
to people who confront a very complex world,
and it's a world where we've had to develop
new mental habits, new habits of mind.
And these include things like
clothing that concrete world with classification,
introducing abstractions that we try to make
logically consistent,
and also taking the hypothetical seriously,
that is, wondering about what might have been
rather than what is.
Now, this dramatic change was drawn to my attention
through massive I.Q. gains over time,
and these have been truly massive.
That is, we don't just get a few more questions right
on I.Q. tests.
We get far more questions right on I.Q. tests
than each succeeding generation
back to the time that they were invented.
Indeed, if you score the people a century ago
against modern norms,
they would have an average I.Q. of 70.
If you score us against their norms,
we would have an average I.Q. of 130.
Now this has raised all sorts of questions.
Were our immediate ancestors
on the verge of mental retardation?
Because 70 is normally the score for mental retardation.
Or are we on the verge of all being gifted?
Because 130 is the cutting line for giftedness.
Now I'm going to try and argue for a third alternative
that's much more illuminating than either of those,
and to put this into perspective,
let's imagine that a Martian came down to Earth
and found a ruined civilization.
And this Martian was an archaeologist,
and they found scores, target scores,
that people had used for shooting.
And first they looked at 1865,
and they found that in a minute,
people had only put one bullet in the bullseye.
And then they found, in 1898,
that they'd put about five bullets in the bullseye in a minute.
And then about 1918 they put a hundred bullets in the bullseye.
And initially, that archaeologist would be baffled.
They would say, look, these tests were designed
to find out how much people were steady of hand,
how keen their eyesight was,
whether they had control of their weapon.
How could these performances have escalated
to this enormous degree?
Well we now know, of course, the answer.
If that Martian looked at battlefields,
they would find that people had only muskets
at the time of the Civil War
and that they had repeating rifles
at the time of the Spanish-American War,
and then they had machine guns
by the time of World War I.
And, in other words, it was the equipment
that was in the hands of the average soldier
that was responsible, not greater keenness of eye
or steadiness of hand.
Now what we have to imagine is the mental artillery
that we have picked up over those hundred years,
and I think again that another thinker will help us here,
and that's Luria.
Luria looked at people
just before they entered the scientific age,
and he found that these people
were resistant to classifying the concrete world.
They wanted to break it up
into little bits that they could use.
He found that they were resistant
to deducing the hypothetical,
to speculating about what might be,
and he found finally that they didn't deal well
with abstractions or using logic on those abstractions.
Now let me give you a sample of some of his interviews.
He talked to the head man of a person
in rural Russia.
They'd only had, as people had in 1900,
about four years of schooling.
And he asked that particular person,
what do crows and fish have in common?
And the fellow said, "Absolutely nothing."
You know, I can eat a fish. I can't eat a crow.
A crow can peck at a fish.
A fish can't do anything to a crow."
And Luria said, "But aren't they both animals?"
And he said, "Of course not.
One's a fish.
The other is a bird."
And he was interested, effectively,
in what he could do with those concrete objects.
And then Luria went to another person,
and he said to them,
"There are no camels in Germany.
Hamburg is a city in Germany.
Are there camels in Hamburg?"
And the fellow said,
"Well, if it's large enough, there ought to be camels there."
And Luria said, "But what do my words imply?"
And he said, "Well, maybe it's a small village,
and there's no room for camels."
In other words, he was unwilling to treat this
as anything but a concrete problem,
and he was used to camels being in villages,
and he was quite unable to use the hypothetical,
to ask himself what if there were no camels in Germany.
A third interview was conducted
with someone about the North Pole.
And Luria said, "At the North Pole, there is always snow.
Wherever there is always snow, the bears are white.
What color are the bears at the North Pole?"
And the response was, "Such a thing
is to be settled by testimony.
If a wise person came from the North Pole
and told me the bears were white,
I might believe him,
but every bear that I have seen is a brown bear."
Now you see again, this person has rejected
going beyond the concrete world
and analyzing it through everyday experience,
and it was important to that person
what color bears were --
that is, they had to hunt bears.
They weren't willing to engage in this.
One of them said to Luria,
"How can we solve things that aren't real problems?
None of these problems are real.
How can we address them?"
Now, these three categories --
classification,
using logic on abstractions,
taking the hypothetical seriously --
how much difference do they make in the real world
beyond the testing room?
And let me give you a few illustrations.
First, almost all of us today get a high school diploma.
That is, we've gone from four to eight years of education
to 12 years of formal education,
and 52 percent of Americans
have actually experienced some type of tertiary education.
Now, not only do we have much more education,
and much of that education is scientific,
and you can't do science without classifying the world.
You can't do science without proposing hypotheses.
You can't do science without making it logically consistent.
And even down in grade school, things have changed.
In 1910, they looked at the examinations
that the state of Ohio gave to 14-year-olds,
and they found that they were all
for socially valued concrete information.
They were things like,
what are the capitals of the 44 or 45 states
that existed at that time?
When they looked at the exams
that the state of Ohio gave in 1990,
they were all about abstractions.
They were things like,
why is the largest city of a state rarely the capital?
And you were supposed to think, well,
the state legislature was rural-controlled,
and they hated the big city,
so rather than putting the capital in a big city,
they put it in a county seat.
They put it in Albany rather than New York.
They put it in Harrisburg rather than Philadelphia.
And so forth.
So the tenor of education has changed.
We are educating people to take the hypothetical seriously,
to use abstractions, and to link them logically.
What about employment?
Well, in 1900, three percent of Americans
practiced professions that were cognitively demanding.
Only three percent were lawyers or doctors or teachers.
Today, 35 percent of Americans
practice cognitively demanding professions,
not only to the professions proper like lawyer
or doctor or scientist or lecturer,
but many, many sub-professions
having to do with being a technician,
a computer programmer.
A whole range of professions now make cognitive demands.
And we can only meet the terms of employment
in the modern world by being cognitively
far more flexible.
And it's not just that we have many more people
in cognitively demanding professions.
The professions have been upgraded.
Compare the doctor in 1900,
who really had only a few tricks up his sleeve,
with the modern general practitioner or specialist,
with years of scientific training.
Compare the banker in 1900,
who really just needed a good accountant
and to know who was trustworthy in the local community
for paying back their mortgage.
Well, the merchant bankers who brought the world to their knees
may have been morally remiss,
but they were cognitively very agile.
They went far beyond that 1900 banker.
They had to look at computer projections
for the housing market.
They had to get complicated CDO-squared
in order to bundle debt together
and make debt look as if it were actually a profitable asset.
They had to prepare a case to get rating agencies
to give it a AAA,
though in many cases, they had virtually bribed the rating agencies.
And they also, of course, had to get people
to accept these so-called assets
and pay money for them
even though they were highly vulnerable.
Or take a farmer today.
I take the farm manager of today as very different
from the farmer of 1900.
So it hasn't just been the spread
of cognitively demanding professions.
It's also been the upgrading of tasks
like lawyer and doctor and what have you
that have made demands on our cognitive faculties.
But I've talked about education and employment.
Some of the habits of mind that we have developed
over the 20th century
have paid off in unexpected areas.
I'm primarily a moral philosopher.
I merely have a holiday in psychology,
and what interests me in general is moral debate.
Now over the last century,
in developed nations like America,
moral debate has escalated
because we take the hypothetical seriously,
and we also take universals seriously
and look for logical connections.
When I came home in 1955 from university
at the time of Martin Luther King,
a lot of people came home at that time
and started having arguments with their parents and grandparents.
My father was born in 1885,
and he was mildly racially biased.
As an Irishman, he hated the English so much
he didn't have much emotion for anyone else.
(Laughter)
But he did have a sense that black people were inferior.
And when we said to our parents and grandparents,
"How would you feel if tomorrow morning you woke up black?"
they said that is the dumbest thing you've ever said.
Who have you ever known who woke up in the morning --
(Laughter) --
that turned black?
In other words, they were fixed in the concrete
mores and attitudes they had inherited.
They would not take the hypothetical seriously,
and without the hypothetical,
it's very difficult to get moral argument off the ground.
You have to say, imagine you were
in Iran, and imagine that your relatives
all suffered from collateral damage
even though they had done no wrong.
How would you feel about that?
And if someone of the older generation says,
well, our government takes care of us,
and it's up to their government to take care of them,
they're just not willing to take the hypothetical seriously.
Or take an Islamic father whose daughter has been raped,
and he feels he's honor-bound to kill her.
Well, he's treating his mores
as if they were sticks and stones and rocks that he had inherited,
and they're unmovable in any way by logic.
They're just inherited mores.
Today we would say something like,
well, imagine you were knocked unconscious and sodomized.
Would you deserve to be killed?
And he would say, well that's not in the Koran.
That's not one of the principles I've got.
Well you, today, universalize your principles.
You state them as abstractions and you use logic on them.
If you have a principle such as,
people shouldn't suffer unless they're guilty of something,
then to exclude black people
you've got to make exceptions, don't you?
You have to say, well, blackness of skin,
you couldn't suffer just for that.
It must be that blacks are somehow tainted.
And then we can bring empirical evidence to bear, can't we,
and say, well how can you consider all blacks tainted
when St. Augustine was black and Thomas Sowell is black.
And you can get moral argument off the ground, then,
because you're not treating moral principles as concrete entities.
You're treating them as universals,
to be rendered consistent by logic.
Now how did all of this arise out of I.Q. tests?
That's what initially got me going on cognitive history.
If you look at the I.Q. test,
you find the gains have been greatest in certain areas.
The similarities subtest of the Wechsler
is about classification,
and we have made enormous gains
on that classification subtest.
There are other parts of the I.Q. test battery
that are about using logic on abstractions.
Some of you may have taken Raven's Progressive Matrices,
and it's all about analogies.
And in 1900, people could do simple analogies.
That is, if you said to them, cats are like wildcats.
What are dogs like?
They would say wolves.
But by 1960, people could attack Raven's
on a much more sophisticated level.
If you said, we've got two squares followed by a triangle,
what follows two circles?
They could say a semicircle.
Just as a triangle is half of a square,
a semicircle is half of a circle.
By 2010, college graduates, if you said
two circles followed by a semicircle,
two sixteens followed by what,
they would say eight, because eight is half of 16.
That is, they had moved so far from the concrete world
that they could even ignore
the appearance of the symbols that were involved in the question.
Now, I should say one thing that's very disheartening.
We haven't made progress on all fronts.
One of the ways in which we would like to deal
with the sophistication of the modern world
is through politics,
and sadly you can have humane moral principles,
you can classify, you can use logic on abstractions,
and if you're ignorant of history and of other countries,
you can't do politics.
We've noticed, in a trend among young Americans,
that they read less history and less literature
and less material about foreign lands,
and they're essentially ahistorical.
They live in the bubble of the present.
They don't know the Korean War from the war in Vietnam.
They don't know who was an ally of America in World War II.
Think how different America would be
if every American knew that this is the fifth time
Western armies have gone to Afghanistan to put its house in order,
and if they had some idea of exactly what had happened
on those four previous occasions.
(Laughter)
And that is, they had barely left,
and there wasn't a trace in the sand.
Or imagine how different things would be
if most Americans knew that we had been lied
into four of our last six wars.
You know, the Spanish didn't sink the battleship Maine,
the Lusitania was not an innocent vessel
but was loaded with munitions,
the North Vietnamese did not attack the Seventh Fleet,
and, of course, Saddam Hussein hated al Qaeda
and had nothing to do with it,
and yet the administration convinced 45 percent of the people
that they were brothers in arms,
when he would hang one from the nearest lamppost.
But I don't want to end on a pessimistic note.
The 20th century has shown enormous cognitive reserves
in ordinary people that we have now realized,
and the aristocracy was convinced
that the average person couldn't make it,
that they could never share their mindset
or their cognitive abilities.
Lord Curzon once said
he saw people bathing in the North Sea,
and he said, "Why did no one tell me
what white bodies the lower orders have?"
As if they were a reptile.
Well, Dickens was right and he was wrong. [Correction: Rudyard Kipling]
[Kipling] said, "The colonel's lady and Judy O'Grady
are sisters underneath the skin."
(Applause)
コツ:単語をクリックしてすぐ意味を調べられます!

読み込み中…

【TED】ジェームズ・フリン: なぜ祖父母世代よりもIQが高いのか (Why our IQ levels are higher than our grandparents' | James Flynn)

46766 タグ追加 保存
CUChou 2015 年 4 月 13 日 に公開
お勧め動画
  1. 1. クリック一つで単語を検索

    右側のスプリクトの単語をクリックするだけで即座に意味が検索できます。

  2. 2. リピート機能

    クリックするだけで同じフレーズを何回もリピート可能!

  3. 3. ショートカット

    キーボードショートカットを使うことによって勉強の効率を上げることが出来ます。

  4. 4. 字幕の表示/非表示

    日・英のボタンをクリックすることで自由に字幕のオンオフを切り替えられます。

  5. 5. 動画をブログ等でシェア

    コードを貼り付けてVoiceTubeの動画再生プレーヤーをブログ等でシェアすることが出来ます!

  6. 6. 全画面再生

    左側の矢印をクリックすることで全画面で再生できるようになります。

  1. クイズ付き動画

    リスニングクイズに挑戦!

  1. クリックしてメモを表示

  1. UrbanDictionary 俚語字典整合查詢。一般字典查詢不到你滿意的解譯,不妨使用「俚語字典」,或許會讓你有滿意的答案喔